BODILY CHANGES
IN PAIN, HUNGER,
FEAR AND RAGE
AN ACCOUNT OF RECENT RESEARCHES INTO THE FUNCTION OF
EMOTIONAL EXCITEMENT
BY
WALTER B. CANNON, M.D., C.B.
GEORGE HIGGINSON, PROFESSOR OF PHYSIOLOGY IN
HARVARD UNIVERSITY
NEW YORK AND LONDON
D. APPLETOX AND COMPANY
1927
COPYRIGHT, 1915, 1920, BY
D. APPLETON AND COMPANY
Printed in the United States of America,
TO MY COLLABORATORS IN THESE RESEARCHES
DANIEL DE LA PAZ
ALFRED T. SHOHL
WADE S. WRIGHT
ARTHUR L. WASHBURN
HENRY LYMAN
LEONARD B. NICE
CHARLES M. GRUBER
HOWARD OSGOOD
HORACE GRAY
WALTER L. MENDENHALL
WITH PLEASANT MEMORIES OF OUR
WORK TOGETHER
PREFACE
Fear, rage and pain, and the pangs of hunger are all
primitive experiences which human beings share with the lower animals.
These experiences are properly classed as among the most powerful that
determine the action of men and beasts. A knowledge of the conditions
which attend these experiences, therefore, is of general and
fundamental importance in the interpretation of behavior.
During the past four years there has been conducted, in the
Harvard Physiological Laboratory, a series of investigations concerned
with the bodily changes which occur in conjunction with pain, hunger
and the major emotions. A group of remarkable alterations in the bodily
economy have been discovered, all of which can reasonably be regarded
as responses that are nicely adapted to the individual's welfare and
preservation.
Because these physiological adaptations are interesting both
in themselves and in their interpretation, not only to physiologists
and psychologists, but to others as well, it has seemed worth while to
gather together in convenient form the original accounts of the
experiments, which have been published in various American medical and
physiological journals. I have, however, attempted to arrange the
results and discussions in an orderly and consecutive manner, and I
have tried also to eliminate or incidentally to explain the technical
terms, so that the exposition will be easily understood by any
intelligent reader even though not trained in the medical sciences.
My first interest in the conditions attending pain, hunger
and strong emotional states was stimulated during the course of a
previous series of researches on the motor activities of the alimentary
canal. A summary of these researches appeared in 1911, under the title,
"The Mechanical Factors of Digestion." The studies recorded in the
present volume may be regarded as a natural sequence of observations on
the influence of emotional states on the digestive process, which were
reported in that volume.
W. B. CANNON.
Boston, Mass.
CONTENTS
CHAPTER I
THE EFFECT OF THE EMOTIONS ON DIGESTION
Emotions favorable to normal secretion of the digestive
juices - Emotions unfavorable to normal secretion of the digestive
juices - Emotions favorable and unfavorable to contractions of
the stomach and intestines - The disturbing effect of pain on digestion. -
5 - 10
CHAPTER II
THE GENERAL ORGANIZATION OF THE VISCERAL NERVES CONCERNED IN
EMOTIONS
The outlying neurones - The three divisions of the
outlying neurones - The extensive distribution of neurones of the
"sympathetic" or thoracico-lumbar division and their arrangement for diffuse action
- The arrangement of neurones of the cranial and sacral divisions for
specific action - The cranial division a conserver of bodily resources
- The sacral division a group of mechanisms for emptying -
The sympathetic division antagonistic to both the cranial and the sacral
- Neurones of the sympathetic division and adrenal secretion have the
same action. - 10 - 15
CHAPTER III
METHODS OF DEMONSTRATING ADRENAL SECRETION AND ITS NERVOUS
CONTROL
The evidence that splanchnic stimulation induces adrenal
secretion - The question of adrenal secretion in emotional
excitement -
The method of securing blood from near the adrenal veins - The
method of testing the blood for adrenin. - 15 - 19
CHAPTER IV
ADRENAL SECRETION IN STRONG EMOTIONS AND PAIN
The evidence that adrenal secretion is increased in emotional
excitement - The evidence that adrenal secretion is increased by
"painful" stimulation - Confirmation of our results by other
observers.
- 19 - 24
CHAPTER V
THE INCREASE OF BLOOD SUGAR IN PAIN AND GREAT EMOTION
Glycosuria from pain - Emotional glycosuria - The
role of the
adrenal glands in emotional glycosuria . - 24 - 28
CHAPTER VI
IMPROVED CONTRACTION OF FATIGUED MUSCLE AFTER SPLANCHNIC
STIMULATION OF THE ADRENAL GLAND
The nerve-muscle preparation - The splanchnic
preparation -
The effects of splanchnic stimulation on the contraction of fatigued
muscle - The first rise in the muscle record - The
prolonged rise in
the muscle record - The two factors: arterial pressure and
adrenal
secretion. - 28 - 32
CHAPTER VII
THE EFFECTS ON CONTRACTION OF FATIGUED MUSCLE OF VARYING THE
ARTERIAL
BLOOD PRESSURE
The effect of increasing arterial pressure - The effect
of
decreasing arterial pressure - An explanation of the effects of
varying
the arterial pressure - The value of increased arterial pressure
in
pain and strong emotion. - 32 - 38
CHAPTER VIII
THE SPECIFIC ROLE OF ADRENIN IN COUNTERACTING THE EFFECTS OF
FATIGUE
Variations of the threshold stimulus as a measure of
irritability - The method of determining the threshold stimulus
- The
lessening of neuro-muscular irritability by fatigue - The slow
restoration of fatigued muscle to normal irritability by rest -
The
quick restoration of fatigued muscle to normal irritability by adrenin
- The evidence that the restorative action of adrenin is specific
- The
point of action of adrenin in muscle. - 38 - 46
CHAPTER IX
THE HASTENING OF THE COAGULATION OF BLOOD BY ADRENIN
The graphic method of measuring the coagulation time -
The
effects of subcutaneous injections of adrenin - The effects of
intravenous injections - The hastening of coagulation by adrenin
not a
direct effect on the blood. - 38 - 55
CHAPTER X
THE HASTENING OF COAGULATION OF BLOOD IN PAIN AND GREAT
EMOTION
Coagulation hastened by splanchnic stimulation -
Coagulation
not hastened by splanchnic stimulation if the adrenal glands are absent
- Coagulation hastened by "painful" stimulation -
Coagulation hastened
in emotional excitement. - 55 - 66
CHAPTER XI
THE UTILITY OF THE BODILY CHANGES IN PAIN AND GREAT EMOTION
The reflex nature of bodily responses in pain and the major
emotions, and the useful character of reflexes - The utility of
the
increased blood sugar as a source of muscular energy - The
utility of
increased adrenin in the blood as an antidote to the effects of fatigue
- The question whether adrenin normally secreted inhibits the use
of
sugar in the body - The vascular changes produced by adrenin
favorable
to supreme muscular exertion - The changes in respiratory
function also
favorable to great effort - The effects produced in asphyxia
similar to
those produced in pain and excitement - The utility of rapid
coagulation in preventing loss of blood . - 66 -75
CHAPTER XII
THE ENERGIZING INFLUENCE OF EMOTIONAL EXCITEMENT
"Reservoirs of power" - The excitements and energies of
competitive sports Frenzy and endurance in ceremonial and other dances
- The fierce emotions and struggles of battle - The
stimulating
influence of witnesses and of music - The feeling of power.
- 75 - 80
CHAPTER XIII
THE NATURE OF HUNGER
Appetite and hunger - The sensation of hunger -
The theory
that hunger is a general sensation - Weakness of the assumptions
underlying the theory that hunger is a general sensation - Body
need
may exist without hunger - The theory that hunger is of general
origin
does not explain the quick onset and the periodicity of the sensation
-
The theory that hunger is of general origin does not explain the local
reference - Hunger not due to emptiness of the stomach -
Hunger not due
to hydrochloric acid in the empty stomach - Hunger not due to
turgescence of the gastric mucous membrane - Hunger the result of
contractions - The "empty" stomach and intestines contract
-
Observations suggesting that contractions cause hunger - The
concomitance of contractions and hunger in man. - 80 - 91
CHAPTER XIV
THE INTERRELATIONS OF EMOTIONS
Antagonism between emotions expressed in the sympathetic and
in the cranial divisions of the autonomic system - Antagonism
between
emotions expressed in the sympathetic and in the sacral divisions of
the autonomic system - The function of hunger - The
similarity of
visceral effects in different strong emotions and suggestions as to its
psychological significance. - 91 - 96
CHAPTER XV
ALTERNATIVE SATISFACTIONS FOR THE FIGHTING EMOTIONS
Support for the militarist estimate of the strength of the
fighting emotions and instincts - Growing opposition to the
fighting
emotions and instincts as displayed in war - The desirability of
preserving the martial virtues - Moral substitutes for warfare
-
Physical substitutes for warfare - The significance of
international
athletic competitions. - 96 - 101
A LIST OF PUBLISHED RESEARCHES FROM THE PHYSIOLOGICAL
LABORATORY IN HARVARD UNIVERSITY - 101
BODILY CHANGES IN PAIN,
HUNGER, FEAR AND RAGE
CHAPTER I
THE EFFECT OF THE EMOTIONS ON DIGESTION
The doctrine of human development from subhuman, antecedents
has done much to unravel the complex nature of man. As a means of
interpretation this doctrine has been directed chiefly toward the
solving of puzzles in the peculiarities of anatomical structure. Thus
arrangements in the human body, which are without obvious utility,
receive rational explanation as being vestiges of parts useful in or
characteristic of remote ancestors parts retained in man because of
agelong racial inheritance. This mode of interpretation has proved
applicable also in accounting for functional peculiarities. Expressive
actions and gestures the facial appearance in anger, for example
observed in children and in widely distinct races, are found to be
innate, and are best explained as the retention in human beings of
responses which are similar in character in lower
animals.
From this point of view biology has contributed much to
clarify our ideas regarding the motives of human behavior. The social
philosophies which prevailed during the past century either assumed
that conduct was determined by a calculated search for pleasure and
avoidance of pain or they ascribed it to a vague and undefined faculty
named the conscience or the moral sense. Comparative study of the
behavior of men and of lower animals under various circumstances,
however, especially with the purpose of learning the source of
prevailing impulses, is revealing the inadequacy of the theories of the
older psychologists. More and more it is appearing that in men of all
races and in most of the higher animals, the springs of action are to
be found in the influence of certain emotions which express themselves
in characteristic instinctive acts. The role which these fundamental
responses in the higher organisms play in the bodily economy has
received little attention. As a realm for investigation the bodily
changes in emotional excitement have been left by the physiologists to
the philosophers and psychologists and to the students of natural
history. These students, however, have usually had too slight
experience in the detailed examination of bodily functions to permit
them to follow the clues which superficial observation might present.
In consequence our knowledge of emotional states has been meagre. There
are, of course, many surface manifestations of excitement. The
contraction of blood vessels with resulting pallor, the pouring out of
"cold sweat," the stopping of saliva-flow so that the "tongue cleaves
to the roof of the mouth," the dilation of the pupils, the rising of
the hairs, the rapid beating of the heart, the hurried respiration, the
trembling and twitching of the muscles, especially those about the lips
all these bodily changes are well recognized accompaniments of pain and
great emotional disturbance, such as fear, horror and deep disgust. But
these disturbances of the even routine of life, which have been
commonly noted, are mainly superficial and therefore readily
observable. Even the increased rapidity of the heart beat is noted at
the surface in the pulsing of the arteries. There are, however, other
organs, hidden deep in the body, which do not reveal so obviously as
the structures near or in the skin, the disturbances of action which
attend states of intense feeling. Special methods must be used to
determine whether these deep-lying organs also are included in the
complex of an emotional* agitation.
* In the use of the term "emotion" the meaning here is
not restricted to violent affective states, but includes "feelings" and
other affective experiences. At times, also, in order to avoid awkward
expressions, the term is used in the popular manner, as if the
"feeling" caused the bodily change.
Among the organs that are affected to an important degree by
feelings are those concerned with digestion. And the relations of
feelings to the activities of the alimentary canal are of particular
interest, because recent investigations have shown that not only are
the first stages of the digestive process normally started by the
pleasurable taste and smell and sight of food, but also that pain and
great emotional excitement can seriously interfere with the starting of
the process or its continuation after it has been started. Thus there
may be a conflict of feelings and of their bodily accompaniments a
conflict the interesting bearing of which we shall consider later.
EMOTIONS FAVORABLE TO NORMAL SECRETION OF THE DIGESTIVE
JUICES
The feelings or affective states favorable to the digestive
functions have been studied fruitfully by Pawlow,1 of Petrograd,
through ingenious experiments on dogs. By the use of careful surgical
methods he was able to make a side pouch of a part of the stomach, the
cavity of which was wholly separate from the main cavity
in which the food was received. This pouch was supplied in a
normal manner with nerves and
blood vessels, and as it opened to the surface of the body,
the amount and character of the gastric juice secreted by it under
various conditions could be accurately determined. Secretion by that
part of the stomach wall which was included in the pouch was
representative of the secretory activities* of the entire stomach. The
arrangement was particularly advantageous in providing the gastric
juice unmixed with food. In some of
the animals thus operated upon an opening was also made in
the esophagus so that when the food was swallowed, it did not pass to
the stomach but dropped out on the way. All the pleasures of eating
were thus experienced, and there was no necessity of stopping because
of a sense of fulness. This process was called "sham feeding."
The well-being of these animals was carefully attended to,
they lived the normal life of dogs, and in the course of months and
years became the pets of the laboratory.
By means of sham feeding Pawlow showed that the chewing and
swallowing of food which the dogs relished resulted, after a delay of
about five minutes, in a flow of natural gastric juice from the side
pouch of the stomach a flow which persisted as long as the dog chewed
and swallowed the food, and continued for some time after eating
ceased. Evidently the presence of food in the stomach is not a prime
condition for gastric secretion. And since the flow occurred only when
the dogs had an appetite, and the material presented to them was
agreeable, the conclusion was justified that this was a true psychic
secretion.
The mere sight or smell of a favorite food may start the
pouring out of gastric juice, as was noted many years ago by Bidder and
Schmidt 2 in a hungry dog which had a fistulous opening through the
body wall into the stomach. This observation, reported in 1852, was
confirmed later by Schiff and also still later by Pawlow. That the
mouth "waters" with a flow of saliva when palatable food is seen or
smelled has long been
such common knowledge that the expression, "It makes my mouth
water," is at once recognized as the highest testimony to the
attractiveness of an appetizing dish. That the stomach also "waters" in
preparation for digesting the food which is to be taken is clearly
proved by the above cited observations on the dog.
The importance of the initial psychic secretion of saliva for
further digestion is indicated when, in estimating the function of
taste for the pleasures of appetite, we realize that materials can be
tasted only when dissolved in the mouth and thereby brought into
relation with the taste organs.
The saliva which "waters" the mouth assures the dissolving of
dry but soluble food even when it is taken in large amount. The
importance of the initial psychic secretion of gastric juice is made
clear by the fact that continuance of the flow of this juice during
digestion is provided by the action of its acid or its digestive
products on the mucous membrane of the pyloric end of the stomach, and
that secretion of the pancreatic juice and bile are called forth by the
action of this same acid on the mucous membrane of the duodenum. The
proper starting of the digestive process, therefore, is conditioned by
the satisfactions of the palate, and
the consequent flow of the first digestive fluids.
The facts brought out experimentally in studies on lower
animals are doubtless true also of man. Not very infrequently, because
of the accidental swallowing of corrosive substances, the esophagus is
so injured that, when it heals, the sides grow together and the tube is
closed. Under these circumstances an opening has to be made into the
stomach through the side of the body and then the individual chews his
food in the usual
manner, but ejects it from his mouth into a tube which is
passed through the gastric opening. The food thus goes from mouth to
stomach through a tube outside the chest instead of inside the chest.
As long ago as 1878, Eichet,3 who had occasion to study a girl whose
esophagus was closed and who was fed through a gastric fistula,
reported that whenever the girl chewed or tasted a highly sapid
substance, such as sugar or lemon juice, while the stomach was empty,
there flowed from the fistula a considerable quantity of gastric
juice. A number of later observers 4 have had similar cases
in human beings, especially in children, and have reported in detail
results which correspond remarkably with those obtained in the
laboratory. Hornborg 4 found that when the little boy whom he studied
chewed agreeable food a more or less active secretion of gastric juice
invariably started, whereas the chewing of an indifferent substance, as
gutta-percha, was followed by no secretion. All these observations
clearly demonstrate that the normal flow of the
first digestive fluids, the saliva and the gastric juice, is
favored by the pleasurable feelings which accompany the taste and smell
of food during mastication, or which are roused in anticipation of
eating when choice morsels are seen or smelled.
These facts are of fundamental importance in the serving of
food, especially when, through illness, the appetite is fickle. The
degree of daintiness with which nourishment is served, the little
attentions to esthetic details the arrangement of the dishes, the small
portions of food, the flower beside the plate all may help to render
food pleasing to the eye and savory to the nostrils and may be the
deciding factors in determining
whether the restoration of strength is to begin or not.
EMOTIONS UNFAVORABLE TO THE NORMAL SECRETION OF THE DIGESTIVE
JUICES
The conditions favorable to proper digestion are wholly
abolished when unpleasant feelings such as vexation and worry and
anxiety, or great emotions such as anger and fear, are allowed to
prevail. This fact, so far as the salivary secretion is concerned, has
long been known. The dry mouth of the anxious person called upon to
speak in public is a common instance; and the "ordeal of rice," as
employed in India, was a practical
utilization of the knowledge that excitement is capable of
inhibiting the salivary flow. When several persons were suspected of
crime, the consecrated rice was given to them all to chew, and after a
short time it was spit out upon the leaf of the sacred fig tree. If
anyone ejected it dry, that was taken as proof that fear of being
discovered had stopped the secretion, and consequently he was adjudged
guilty.5
What has long been recognized as true of the secretion of
saliva has been proved true also of the secretion of gastric juice. For
example, Hornborg was unable to confirm in his little patient with a
gastric fistula the observation by Pawlow that when hunger is present
the mere seeing of food results in a flow of gastric juice.
Hornborg explained the difference between his and Pawlow's
results by the different ways in which the boy and the dogs faced the
situation. When food was shown, but withheld, the hungry dogs were all
eagerness to secure it, and the juice very soon began to flow. The boy,
on the contrary, became vexed when he could not eat at once, and began
to cry; then no secretion appeared. Bogen also has reported the
instance of a child with closed esophagus and gastric fistula, who
sometimes fell into such a passion in consequence of vain hoping for
food that the giving of the food, after the child was calmed, was not
followed by any flow of the secretion.
The inhibitory influence of excitement has also been seen in
lower animals under laboratory conditions.
Le Conte6 declares that in studying gastric secretion it is
necessary to avoid all circumstances likely to provoke emotional
reactions. In the fear which dogs manifest when first brought into
strange surroundings he found that activity of the gastric glands may
be completely suppressed. The suppression occurred even if
the dog had eaten freely and was then disturbed as, for
example, by being tied to a table. When the animals became accustomed
to the experimental procedure, it no longer had an inhibitory effect.
The studies of Bickel and Sasaki7 confirm and define more precisely
this inhibitory effect of strong emotion on gastric secretion. They
observed the inhibition on a dog with an esophageal fistula, and with a
side pouch of the
stomach, which, as in Pawlow's experiments, opened only to
the exterior. In this dog Bickel and Sasaki noted, as Pawlow had, that
sham feeding was attended by a copious flow of gastric juice, a true
psychic secretion, resulting from the pleasurable taste of the food. In
a typical instance the sham feeding lasted five minutes, and the
secretion continued for twenty minutes, during which time 66.7 cubic
centimeters of pure gastric juice were produced.
On another day a cat was brought into the presence of the
dog, whereupon the dog flew into a great fury. The cat was soon
removed, and the dog pacified. Now the dog was again given the sham
feeding for five minutes. In spite of the fact that the animal was
hungry and ate eagerly, there was no secretion worthy of mention.
During a period of twenty minutes, corresponding to the previous
observation, only9 cubic centimeters of acid fluid were produced, and
this was rich in mucus. It is evident that in the dog, as in the boy
observed by Bogen, strong emotions can so profoundly disarrange the
mechanisms of secretion that the pleasurable excitation which
accompanies the taking of food cannot cause the normal flow.
On another occasion Bickel and Sasaki started gastric
secretion in the dog by sham feeding, and when the flow of gastric
juice had reached a certain height, the dog was infuriated for five
minutes by the presence of the cat. During the next fifteen minutes
there appeared only a few drops of a very mucous secretion. Evidently
in this instance a physiological process, started as an accompaniment
of a psychic state quietly pleasurable in character, was almost
entirely stopped after another psychic state violent in character.
It is noteworthy that in both the favorable and unfavorable
results of the emotional excitement illustrated in Bickel and Sasaki's
dog the effects persisted long after the removal of the exciting
condition. This fact, in its favorable aspect, Bickel 8 was able to
confirm in a girl with esophageal and gastric fistulas; the gastric
secretion long outlasted the period of eating, although no food entered
the stomach. The influences
unfavorable to digestion, however, are stronger than those
which promote it. And evidently, if the digestive process, because of
emotional disturbance, is for some time inhibited, the swallowing of
food which must lie stagnant in the stomach is a most irrational
procedure. If a child has experienced an outburst of passion, it is
well not to urge the taking of nourishment soon afterwards. Macbeth's
advice that "good digestion
wait on appetite and health on both," is now well-founded
physiology.
Other digestive glands than the salivary and the gastric may
be checked in emotional excitement. Recently Oechsler9 has reported
that in such psychic disturbances as were shown by Bickel and Sasaki to
be accompanied by suppressed secretion of the gastric juice, the
secretion of pancreatic juice may be stopped, and the flow of bile
definitely checked. All the means of bringing about chemical changes in
the food may
be thus temporarily abolished.
EMOTIONS FAVORABLE AND UNFAVORABLE TO THE CONTRACTIONS OF THE
STOMACH AND INTESTINES
The secretions of the digestive glands and the chemical
changes wrought by them are of little worth unless the food is carried
onward through the alimentary canal into fresh regions of digestion and
is thoroughly exposed to the intestinal wall for absorption. In.
studying these mechanical aspects of digestion I was led to infer 10
that just as there is a psychic secretion, so likewise there is
probably a "psychic tone" or "psychic
contraction" of the gastro-intestinal muscles as a result of
taking food. For if the vagus nerve supply to the stomach is cut
immediately before an animal takes food, the usual
contractions of the gastric wall, as seen by the Rontgen rays, do not
occur; but if these nerves are cut after food has been eaten
with relish, the contractions which have started continue without
cessation. The nerves in both conditions were severed under anesthesia,
so that no element of pain entered into the experiments. In the absence
of hunger,
which in itself provides a contracted stomach, 11 the
pleasurable taking of food may, therefore, be a primary condition for
the appearance of natural contractions of the gastro-intestinal canal.
Again just as the secretory activities of the stomach are
unfavorably influenced by strong emotions, so also are the movements of
the stomach; and, indeed, the movements of almost the entire alimentary
canal are wholly stopped during great excitement. In my earliest
observations on the movements of the stomach 12 I had difficulty
because in some animals the waves of contraction were perfectly
evident, while in others
there was no sign of activity. Several weeks passed before I
discovered that this difference was associated with a difference of
sex. In order to be observed with Rontgen rays the animals were
restrained in a holder. Although the holder was comfortable, the male
cats, particularly the young males, were restive and excited on being
fastened to it, and under these circumstances gastric peristaltic waves
were absent; the female
cats, especially if elderly, usually submitted with calmness
to the restraint, and in them the waves
had their normal occurrence. Once a female with kittens
turned from her state of quiet contentment
to one of apparent restless anxiety. The movements of the
stomach immediately stopped, the gastric wall became wholly relaxed,
and only after the animal had been petted and began to purr did the
moving waves start again on their course. By covering the cat's mouth
and nose with the fingers until a slight distress of breathing is
produced, the stomach contractions can be stopped at will. In the cat,
therefore, any sign
of rage or fear, such as was seen in dogs by Le Conte and by
Bickel and Sasaki, was accompanied by a total abolition of the
movements of the stomach. Even indications of slight anxiety may be
attended by complete absence of the churning waves. In a vigorous young
male cat I have watched the stomach for more than an hour by means of
the Rontgen rays, and during that time not the slightest beginning of
peristaltic activity
appeared; yet the only visible indication of excitement in
the animal was a continued quick twitching of the tail to and fro. What
is true of the cat I have found true also of the rabbit, dog and
guinea-pig 13 very mild emotional disturbances are attended by
abolition of peristalsis.
The observations on the rabbit have been confirmed by Auer,
14 who found that the handling of the animal incidental to fastening it
gently to a holder stopped gastric peristalsis for a variable length of
time. And if the animal was startled for any reason, or struggled
excitedly, peristalsis was again abolished. The observations
on the dog also have been confirmed; Lommel 15 found that
small dogs in strange surroundings might have no contractions of the
stomach for two or three hours. And whenever the animals showed any
indications of being uncomfortable or distressed, the contractions were
inhibited and the discharge of contents from the
stomach checked.
Like the peristaltic waves in the stomach, the peristalsis
and the kneading movements (segmentation)
in the small intestine, and the reversed peristalsis in the
large intestine all cease whenever the observed animal shows signs of
emotional excitement.
There is no doubt that just as the secretory activity of the
stomach is affected in a similar fashion in man and in lower animals,
so likewise gastric and intestinal peristaltic waves are stopped in man
as they are stopped in lower animals, by worry and anxiety and the
stronger affective states. The conditions of mental discord
may thus give rise to a sense of gastric inertia. For
example, a patient described by Müller 16 testified that anxiety
was
always accompanied by a feeling of weight, as if the food remained in
the stomach. Every addition of food caused an increase of the trouble.
Strong emotional states in this instance led almost always to gastric
distress, which persisted, according to the grade and the duration of
the psychic disturbance, between a half-hour and several days. The
patient was not hysterical or neurasthenic, but was a very
sensitive woman deeply affected by moods.
The feeling of heaviness in the stomach, mentioned in the
foregoing case, is not uncommonly complained of by nervous persons, and
may be due to stagnation of the contents. That such stagnation occurs
is shown by the following instance. A refined and sensitive woman, who
had had digestive difficulties, came with her husband to Boston to be
examined. They went to a hotel for the night. The next morning the
woman appeared at the consultant's office an hour after having eaten a
test meal. An examination of the gastric contents revealed no free
acid, no digestion of the test breakfast, and the presence of a
considerable amount of the supper of the previous evening. The
explanation of this stagnation of the food in the stomach came from the
family doctor, who reported that the husband had made the visit to the
city an occasion for becoming uncontrollably drunk, and that he had by
his escapades given his wife a night of turbulent
anxiety. The second morning, after the woman had had a good
rest, the gastric contents were again examined; the proper acidity was
found, and the test breakfast had been normally digested and
discharged.
These cases are merely illustrative and doubtless can be many
times duplicated in the experience of any physician concerned largely
with digestive disorders. ( Indeed, the opinion has been expressed that
a great majority of the cases of gastric indigestion that come for
treatment are functional in character and of nervous origin. It is the
emotional element that seems most characteristic of these cases. To so
great an extent
is this true that Rosenbach has suggested that as a term to
characterize the cause of the disturbances,
"emotional" dyspepsia is better than "nervous" dyspepsia. 17
THE DISTURBING EFFECT OF PAIN ON DIGESTION
(The advocates of the theory of organic evolution early
pointed out the similarity between the bodily disturbances in pain and
in the major emotions. The alterations of function of internal organs
they could not know about. The general statement, however, that pain
evokes the same changes that are evoked by emotion, is true also of
these deep-lying structures, ) Wertheimer 18 proved many years since
that stimulation of a
sensory nerve in an anesthetized animal such stimulation as
in a conscious animal would induce pain quickly abolished the
contractions of the stomach. And Netschaiev, working in Pawlow's 19
laboratory, showed that excitation of the sensory fibres in the sciatic
nerve for two or three minutes resulted in an inhibition of the
secretion of gastric juice that lasted for several hours.
Similar effects from painful experience have been not uncommonly noted
in human beings. 'Mantegazza, 20 in his account of the physiology of
pain, has cited a number of such examples, and from them he has
concluded that pain interferes with digestion by lessening appetite and
by producing various forms of dyspepsia, with arrest of gastric
digestion, and with vomiting
and diarrhea. The expression, "sickening pain" is testimony
to the power of strong sensory stimulation to upset the digestive
processes profoundly. Vomiting is as likely to follow violent pain as
it is to follow strong emotion. A "sick headache" may be, indeed, a
sequence of events in which the pain from the headache is primary, and
the nausea and other evidences of digestive disorder are secondary.
As the foregoing account has shown, emotional conditions or
"feelings" may be accompanied by
quite opposite effects in the alimentary canal, some highly
favorable to good digestion, some highly disturbing. It is an
interesting fact that the feelings having these antagonistic actions
are typically expressed through nerve supplies which are
correspondingly opposed in their influence on the digestive organs. The
antagonism between these nerve supplies is of fundamental importance in
understanding not only the operation of conditions favorable or
unfavorable to digestion but also in obtaining insight into the
conflicts of emotional states. Since a consideration of the arrangement
and mode of action of these nerves
will establish a firm basis for later analysis and
conclusions, they will next be considered.
REFERENCES
1 Pawlow: The Work of the Digestive Glands, London, 1902.
2 Bidder and Schmidt: Die Verdauungssafte und der
Stoffwechsel, Leipzig, 1852, p. 35.
3 Richet: Journal de 1'Anatomie et de la Physiologie, 1878,
xiv, p. 170.
4 See Hornborg: Skandinavisches Archiv für Physiologie,
1904,
xv, p. 248. Cade and Latarjet: Journal de Physiologie et Pathologie
Generale, 1905, vii, p. 221. Bogen: Archiv für die gesammte
Physiologie, 1907, cxvii, p. 156. Lavenson: Archives of Internal
Medicine, 1909, iv, p. 271.
5 Lea: Superstition and Force, Philadelphia, 1892, p. 344.
6 Le Conte: La Cellule, 1900, xvii, p. 291.
7 Bickel and Sasaki: Deutsche medizinische Wochenschrift,
1905, xxxi, p. 1829.
8 Bickel: Berliner klinische Wochenschrift, 1906, xliii, p.
845.
9 Oechsler: Internationelle Beitrage zur Pathologie und
Therapie der Ernahrungstorungen, 1914, v, p. 1.
10 Cannon: The Mechanical Factors of Digestion, London and
New York, 1911, p. 200.
11 Cannon and Washburn: American Journal of Physiology, 1912,
xxix, p. 441.
12 Cannon: The American Journal of Physiology, 1898, i, p.
38.
13 Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1902, vii, p.
xxii.
14 Auer: American Journal of Physiology, 1907, xviii, p. 356.
15 Lommel: Münchener medizinische Wochenschrift, 1903,
i, p.
1634.
16 Müller: Deutsches Archiv für klinische Medicin,
1907,
1xxxix, p. 434.
17 Rosenbach: Berliner klinische Wochenschrift, 1897, xxxiv,
p. 71
18 Wertheimer: Archives de Physiologic, 1892, xxiv, p. 379.
19 Pawlow: Loc. cit., p. 56.
20 Mantegazza: Fisiologia del Dolore, Florence, 1880, p. 123.
CHAPTER II
THE GENERAL ORGANIZATION OF THE VISCERAL NERVES CONCERNED IN
EMOTIONS
The structures of the alimentary canal which are brought into
activity during the satisfactions of appetite or are checked in their
activity during pain and emotional excitement are either the secreting
digestive glands or the smooth muscle which surrounds the canal. Both
the gland cells and the smooth-muscle cells differ from other cells
which are subject to nervous influence those of striated, or skeletal,
muscle in not being
directly under voluntary control and in being slower in their
response. The muscle connected with the skeleton responds to
stimulation within two or three thousandths of a second; the delay with
gland cells and with smooth muscle is more likely to be measured in
seconds than in fractions of a second.
THE OUTLYING NEURONES
The skeletal muscles receive their nerve supply direct from
the central nervous system, i. e., the nerve fibres distributed to
these muscles are parts of neurones whose cell bodies lie within the
brain or spinal cord. The glands and smooth muscles of the viscera, on
the contrary, are, so far as is now known, never innervated directly
from the central nervous system.* The neurones reaching out from the
brain or spinal cord never come into immediate relation with the gland
or smooth-muscle cells; there are always interposed between
the cerebrospinal neurones and the viscera extra neurones
whose bodies and processes lie wholly outside the central nervous
system. They are represented in dotted lines in Fig. 1. I have
suggested that possibly these outlying neurones act as "transformers,"
modifying the impulses received from the central source (impulses
suited to call forth the quick responses of skeletal muscle), and
adapting these impulses to the peculiar, more
slowly-acting tissues, the secreting cells and visceral
muscle, to which they are distributed.1 The outlying neurones typically
have their cell bodies grouped in ganglia (G's, Fig. 1) which, in the
trunk region, lie along either side of the spinal cord and in the head
region and in the pelvic part of the abdominal cavity are disposed near
the organs which the neurones supply. In some instances these neurones
lie wholly within the
* The special case of the adrenal glands will be
considered
later.
.
structure which they innervate (see e. g., the heart and the
stomach, Fig. 1). In other instances the fibres passing out from the
ganglia the so-called "postganglionic fibres" may traverse long
distances before reaching their destination. The innervation of blood
vessels in the foot by neurones whose cell bodies are in the lower
trunk region is an example of this extensive distribution of the
fibres.
THE THREE DIVISIONS OF THE OUTLYING NEURONES
As suggested above, the outlying neurones are connected with
the brain and spinal cord by neurones whose cell bodies lie within the
central nervous organs. These connecting neurones, represented by
continuous lines in Fig. 1, do not pass out in an uninterrupted series
all along the cerebro-spinal axis. Where the nerves pass out from the
spinal cord to the fore and hind limbs, fibres are not given off to the
ganglia. Thus these connecting or "proganglionic" fibres are separated
into three divisions. In front of the nerve roots for the fore limbs is
the head or cranial division; between the nerve roots for the
fore limbs and those for the hind limbs is the trunk division
(or thoracico-lumbar division, or, in the older terminology,
the "sympathetic system"); and after the nerve roots for the hind limbs
the sacral division.
This system of outlying neurones, with post ganglionic fibres
innervating the viscera, and with preganglionic fibres reaching out to
them from the cerebrospinal system, has been called by Langley, to whom
we are indebted for most of our knowledge of its organization, the autonomic
nervous system. 2 This term indicates that the structures which
the system supplies are not subject to voluntary control, but operate
to a large
degree independently. As we have seen, a highly potent mode
of influencing these structures is through conditions of pain and
emotional excitement. The parts of the autonomic system the cranial,
the sympathetic, and the sacral have a number of peculiarities which
are of prime importance in accounting for the bodily manifestations of
such affective states.
THE EXTENSIVE DISTRIBUTION OF NEURONES OF THE "SYMPATHETIC"
DIVISION AND THEIR ARRANGEMENT FOR DIFFUSE ACTION
The fibres of the sympathetic division differ from those of
the other two divisions in being distributed through the body very
widely. They go to the eyes, causing dilation of the pupils. They go to
the heart and, when stimulated, they cause it to beat rapidly. They
carry impulses to arteries and arterioles of the skin, the abdominal
viscera, and other parts, keeping the smooth muscles of the vessel
walls in a state of slight contraction or tone, and thus serving to
maintain an arterial pressure sufficiently high to meet sudden
demands in any special region; or, in times of special
discharge of impulses, to increase the tone and thus also the arterial
pressure. They are distributed extensively to the smooth muscle
attached to the hairs; and when they cause this muscle to contract, the
hairs are erected. They go to sweat glands, causing the outpouring of
sweat. These fibres pass also to the entire length of the
gastro-intestinal canal. And the inhibition of digestive activity
which, as we have learned, occurs in pain and emotional states, is due
to impulses which are conducted outward by the splanchnic nerves
the preganglionic fibres that reach to the great ganglia in the upper
abdomen (see Fig. 1) and thence are spread by postganglionic fibres all
along the gut. 3 They innervate likewise the genito-urinary tracts,
causing contraction of the smooth muscle of the internal genital
organs, and usually relaxation of the bladder. Finally they affect the
liver, releasing the
storage of material there in a manner which may be of great
service to the body in time of need.
The extensiveness of the distribution of the fibres
of the sympathetic division is one of its most prominent
characteristics.
Another typical feature of the sympathetic division is an
arrangement of neurones for diffuse discharge of the nerve impulses. As
shown diagrammatically in Fig. 1, the preganglionic fibres from the
central nervous system may extend through several of the sympathetic
ganglia and give off in each of them connections to cell bodies of the
outlying neurones. Although the neurones which transmit sensory
impulses from the skin
into spinal cord have similar relations to nerve cells lying
at different levels of the cord, the operation in the two cases is
quite different. In the spinal cord the sensory impulse produces
directed and closely limited effects, as, for example, when reflexes
are being evoked in a "spinal" animal (i. e., an animal with the spinal
cord isolated from the rest of the central nervous system), the left
hind limb is nicely lifted, in response to a harmful stimulus applied
to the left foot, without widespread marked involvement of the rest of
the body in the response. 4 In the action of the sympathetic division,
on the contrary, the connection of single preganglionic fibres with
numerous outlying neurones seems to be not at all arranged for specific
effects in this or that particular region. There are, to be sure, in
different circumstances variations in the degree of activity of
different parts; for example, it is probable that dilation of the pupil
in the cat occurs more readily than erection of the hairs. It may be in
this instance, however, that specially direct pathways to the eye are
present for common use in non-emotional states (in dim light, e. g.),
and that only slight general disturbance in the central nervous system,
therefore, would be necessary to send impulses by these well-worn
courses. Thus for local reasons (dust, e. g.) tears might flow from
excitation of the tear glands by sympathetic impulses, although other
parts innervated by this same division might be but little disturbed.
We have no means of voluntarily wearing these pathways, however, and
both from anatomical and physiological evidence the neurone relations
in the sympathetic division of the autonomic system seem devised for
widespread diffusion of nervous impulses.
THE ARRANGEMENT OF NEURONES OF THE CRANIAL AND SACRAL
DIVISIONS FOR SPECIFIC ACTION
The cranial and sacral autonomic divisions differ from the
sympathetic in having only restricted distribution (see Fig.
1). The third cranial nerves deliver impulses from the brain to ganglia
in which lie the cell bodies of neurones innervating smooth muscle only
in the front of the eyes. The vagus nerves are distributed to the
lungs, heart, stomach, and small intestine. As shown diagrammatically
in Fig. 1, the outlying neurones in the last three of these organs lie
within the organs themselves. By this arrangement, although the
preganglionic fibres of the vagi are extended in various directions to
structures of quite diverse functions, singleness and separateness of
connection of the peripheral organs with the central nervous system is
assured. The same specific relation between efferent fibres and the
viscera is seen in the sacral autonomic. In this division the
preganglionic fibres pass out from the spinal cord to ganglia lying in
close proximity to the distal colon, the bladder, and the external
genitals. And the postganglionic fibres deliver the nerve impulses only
to the nearby organs. Besides these innervations the cranial and sacral
divisions supply individual arteries with "dilator nerves" nerves
causing relaxation of the particular vessels. Quite typically,
therefore, the efferent fibres of the two terminal divisions of the
autonomic differ from those of the mid-division in having few of the
diffuse connections characteristic of the mid-division, and in
innervating distinctively the organs to which they are distributed. The
cranial and sacral preganglionic fibres resemble thus the nerves to
skeletal muscles, and their arrangement provides similar possibilities
of specific and separate action in any part, without action in other
parts.
THE CRANIAL DIVISION A CONSERVER OP BODILY KESOURCES
The cranial autonomic, represented by the vagus nerves, is
the part of the visceral nervous system concerned in the psychic
secretion of the gastric juice. Pawlow showed that when these nerves
are severed psychic secretion is abolished. The cranial nerves to the
salivary glands are similarly the agents for psychic secretion in these
organs, and are known to cause also dilation of the arteries supplying
the glands, so that during activity the glands receive a more abundant
flow of blood. As previously stated (see p. 13), the evidence for a
psychic tonus of the gastro-intestinal musculature rests on a failure
of the normal contractions if the vagi are severed before food is
taken, in contrast to the continuance of the contractions if the nerves
are severed just afterwards. The vagi artificially excited are
wellknown as stimulators of increased tone in the smooth muscle of the
alimentary canal. Aside from these positive effects on the muscles of
the digestive tract and its accessory glands, cranial autonomic fibres
cause contraction of the pupil of the eye, and slowing of the heart
rate. A glance at these various functions of the cranial division
reveals at once that they serve for bodily conservation. By narrowing
the pupil of the eye they shield the retina from excessive light. By
slowing the heart rate, they give the cardiac muscle longer periods for
rest and invigoration. And by providing for the flow of saliva and
gastric juice and by supplying the muscular tone necessary for
contraction of the alimentary canal, they prove fundamentally essential
to the processes of proper digestion and absorption by which
energy-yielding material is taken into the body and stored. To the
cranial division of the visceral nerves, therefore, belongs the quiet
service of building up reserves and fortifying the body against times
of need or stress.
THE SACRAL DIVISION A GROUP OF MECHANISMS FOR EMPTYING
Sacral autonomic fibres cause contraction of the rectum and
distal colon and also contraction of the bladder. In both instances the
effects result reflexly from stretching of the tonically contracted
viscera by their accumulating contents. No affective states precede
this normal action of the sacral division and even those which
accompany or follow are only mildly positive; a feeling of relief
rather than of elation usually attends
the completion of the act of defecation or micturition though
there is testimony to the contrary.
The sacral autonomic fibres also include, however, the nervi
erigentes which bring about engorgement of erectile tissue in the
external genitals.
According to Langley and Anderson 5 the sacral nerves have no
effect on the internal generative organs. The vasa deferentia
and the seminal vesicles whose rhythmic contractions mark the acme of
sexual excitement in the male, and the uterus whose contractions in the
female are probably analogous, are supplied only by lumbar branches
part of the sympathetic division. These branches also act in opposition
to the nervi erigentes and cause constriction of the blood vessels of
the external genitals. The sexual orgasm involves a high degree of
emotional excitement; but it can be rightly considered as essentially a
reflex mechanism; and, again in this instance, distention of tubules,
vesicles, and blood vessels can be found at the beginning of the
incident, and relief from this distension at the end. Although
distention is the commonest occasion for bringing the sacral division
into activity it is not the only occasion. Great emotion, such as is
accompanied by nervous discharges via the sympathetic division, may
also be accompanied by discharges via the sacral fibres. The
involuntary voiding of the bladder and lower gut at times of violent
mental stress is well-known. Veterans of wars testify that just before
the beginning of a battle many of the men have to retire temporarily
from the firing line. And the power of sights and smells and
libidinous thoughts to disturb the regions controlled by the nervi
erigentes proves that this part of the autonomic system also has its
peculiar affective states. The fact that one part of the sacral
division, e. g., the distribution to the bladder, may be in abeyance,
while another part, e. g., the distribution to the rectum, is active,
illustrates again the directive discharge of impulses which has been
previously described as characteristic of the cranial and sacral
portions of the autonomic system.
Like the cranial division, the sacral is engaged in internal
service to the body, in the performance of acts leading immediately to
greater comfort.
THE SYMPATHETIC DIVISION ANTAGONISTIC TO BOTH THE CRANIAL AND
THE SACRAL
As indicated in the foregoing description many of the viscera
are innervated both by the cranial or sacral part of the autonomic and
by the sympathetic. When the mid-part meets either endpart in any
viscus their effects are antagonistic. Thus the cranial supply to
the eye contracts the pupil, the sympathetic dilates it; the cranial
slows the heart, the sympathetic accelerates it; the sacral contracts
the lower part of the large intestine, the sympathetic relaxes it; the
sacral relaxes the exit from the bladder, the sympathetic contracts it.
These opposed effects are indicated in Fig. 1 by + for contraction,
acceleration or increased tone; and by - for inhibition, relaxation, or
decreased tone.*
* The vagus nerve, when artificially stimulated, has a
primary, brief inhibitory effect on the stomach and small intestine;
its main function, however, as already stated, is to produce increased
tone and contraction in these organs. This double action of the vagus
is marked thus, ± , in Fig. 1.
Sherrington has demonstrated that the setting of skeletal
muscles in opposed groups about a joint or system of joints as in
flexors and extensors is associated with an internal organization of
the central nervous system that provides for relaxation of one group of
the opposed muscles when the other group is made to contract.
This "reciprocal innervation of antagonistic muscles," as
Sherrington has called it, 6 is thus a device for orderly action in the
body. As the above description has shown, there are peripheral
oppositions in the viscera corresponding to the oppositions between
flexor and extensor muscles. In all probability these opposed
innervations of the viscera have counterparts in the organization of
neurones in the central nervous system. Sherrington has noticed, and I
can confirm the observation, that even though the sympathetic supply
to the eye is severed and is therefore incapable of causing
dilation of the pupil, nevertheless the pupil dilates in a paroxysm of
anger due, no doubt (because the response is too rapid to be mediated
by the blood stream), to central inhibition of the cranial nerve supply
to the constrictor muscles i. e., an inhibition of the muscles which
naturally oppose the dilator action of the sympathetic. Pain, the major
emotions fear and rage and also intense excitement, are manifested in
the activities of the sympathetic division. When in these states
impulses rush out over the neurones of this division they produce all
the changes typical of sympathetic excitation, such as dilating the
pupils, inhibiting digestion, causing pallor, accelerating the heart,
and various
other well-known effects. The impulses of the sympathetic
neurones, as indicated by their dominance over the digestive process,
are capable of readily overwhelming the conditions established by
neurones of the cranial division of the autonomic system.
NEURONES OF THE SYMPATHETIC DIVISION AND ADRENAL
SECRETION HAVE THE SAME ACTION
Lying anterior to each kidney is a small body the adrenal
gland. It is composed of an external portion or cortex, and a central
portion or medulla. From the medulla can be extracted a substance,
called variously suprarenin, adrenin, epinephrin or "adrenalin,"*
which, in extraordinarily minute amounts, affects the structures
innervated by the sympathetic division of the autonomic system
* The name "adrenalin" is proprietary. "Epinephrin"
and "adrenin" have been suggested as terms free from commercial
suggestions. As adrenin is shorter and more clearly related to the
common adjectival form, adrenal, I have followed Schafer in using
adrenin to designate the substance produced physiologically by the
adrenal glands.
precisely as if they were receiving nervous impulses. For
example, when adrenin is injected into the blood, it will cause pupils
to dilate, hairs to stand erect, blood vessels to be constricted, the
activities of the alimentary canal to be inhibited, and sugar to be
liberated from the liver. These effects are not produced by action of
the substance on the central nervous system, but by direct action on
the organ itself. 7 And the effects occur
even after the structures have been removed from the body and
kept alive artificially.
The adrenals are glands of internal secretion, i. e., like
the thyroid, parathyroid, and pituitary glands, for example; they have
no connection with the surface of the body, and they give out into the
blood the material which they elaborate. The blood is carried away from
each of them by the lumbo-adrenal vein which empties either into the
renal vein or directly into the inferior vena cava just anterior to the
openings of the renal veins. The adrenal glands are supplied by
preganglionic fibres of the autonomic group,8 shown in solid
line in Fig. 1. This seems an exception to the general rule
that gland cells have an outlying neurone between them and the neurones
of the central nervous system. The medulla of the adrenal gland,
however, is composed of modified nerve cells, and may therefore be
regarded as offering exceptional conditions.
The foregoing brief sketch of the organization of the
autonomic system brings out a number of points that should be of
importance as bearing on the nature of the emotions which manifest
themselves in the operations of this system. Thus it is highly probable
that the sympathetic division, because arranged for diffuse discharge,
is likely to be brought into activity as a whole, whereas the sacral
and cranial divisions, arranged for particular action on separate
organs, may operate in parts. Also, because antagonisms exist between
the middle and either end division of the autonomic, affective states
may be classified according
to their expression in the middle or an end division and
these states would be, like the nerves, antagonistic in character. And
finally, since the adrenal glands are innervated by autonomic fibres of
the mid-division, and since adrenal secretion stimulates the 'same
activities that are stimulated nervously by this division, it is
possible that disturbances in the realm of the sympathetic,
although initiated by nervous discharge,
are automatically augmented and prolonged through chemical
effects of the adrenal secretion.
REFERENCES
1 Cannon: The American Journal of Psychology, 1914, xxv, p.
257.
2 For a summary of his studies of the organization of the
autonomic system, see Langley: Ergebnisse der Physiologie, Wiesbaden,
1903, ii 2, p. 818.
3 See Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1905, xiii, p.
xxii.
4 See Sherrington: The Integrative Action of the Nervous
System, New York, 1909, p. 19.
5 Langley and Anderson: Journal of Physiology, 1895, xix, see
pp. 85, 122.
6 Sherrington: Loc. cit., p. 90.
7 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1905, xxxii, p. 426.
8 See Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1913, xlvi, p. 289 ff.
CHAPTER III
METHODS OF DEMONSTRATING ADRENAL SECRETION AND ITS NERVOUS
CONTROL
As stated in the first chapter, the inhibition of gastric
secretion produced by great excitement long outlasts the presence of
the object which evokes the excitement. The dog that was enraged by
seeing a cat for five minutes secreted only a few drops of gastric
juice during the next fifteen minutes. Why did the state of excitation
persist so long after the period of stimulation had ended? This
question, which presented itself to
me while reading Bickel and Sasaki's paper, furnished the
suggestion expressed at the close of the last chapter, that the
excitement might provoke a flow of adrenal secretion, and that the
changes originally induced in the digestive organs by nervous impulses
might be continued by circulating adrenin. The prolongation of the
effect might be thus explained. Whether that idea is correct or not has
not been tested. Its chief service was in leading to an enquiry as to
whether the adrenal glands are in fact stimulated to action
in emotional excitement. The preganglionic fibres passing to
the glands are contained in the splanchnic
nerves. What is the effect of splanchnic stimulation?
THE EVIDENCE THAT SPLANCHNIC STIMULATION INDUCES ADRENAL
SECRETION
It was in 1891 that Jacobi 1 described nerve fibres derived
from the splanchnic trunks which were distributed to the adrenal
glands. Six years later Biedl 2 found that these nerves conveyed
vaso-dilator impulses to the glands, and he suggested that they
probably conveyed also secretory impulses. Evidence in support of this
suggestion was presented the following year by Dreyer,3 who
demonstrated that electrical excitation of the splanchnic nerves
produced in the blood taken from the adrenal veins an increased amount
of a substance having the power of raising arterial blood pressure, and
that this result was independent of accompanying changes in the blood
supply to the glands. The conclusion drawn by Dreyer that this
substance was adrenin has been confirmed in various ways by later
observers. Tscheboksaroff 4 repeated Dreyer's procedure
and found in blood taken from the veins after splanchnic
stimulation evidences of the presence of adrenin that were previously
absent. Asher 5 observed a rise of blood pressure when the glands were
stimulated in such a manner as not to cause constriction of the
arteries the rise was therefore assumed to be due to secreted adrenin.
Dilation of the pupil was used by Meltzer and Joseph 6 to prove
secretory action of the splanchnics
on the adrenal glands; they found that stimulation of the
distal portion of the cut splanchnic nerve caused the pupil to enlarge
an effect characteristic of adrenin circulating in the blood. Elliott 7
repeated this procedure, but made it a more rigorous proof of internal
secretion of the adrenals by noting that the effect failed to appear if
the gland on the stimulated side was removed.
Additional proof was brought by myself and Lyman 8 when we
found that the typical drop in arterial pressure produced in cats by
injecting small amounts of adrenin could be exactly reproduced by
stimulating the splanchnic nerves after the abdominal blood vessels,
which contract when these nerves are excited, were tied so that no
changes in them could occur to influence the rest of the circulation.
The problem of splanchnic influence on the adrenal glands
Elliott attacked by a still different method. Using, as a measure, the
graded effects of graded amounts of adrenin on blood pressure, he was
able to assay the quantity of adrenin in adrenal glands after various
conditions had been allowed to prevail. The tests were made on cats. In
these animals each adrenal gland is supplied only by the splanchnic
fibres of its own side, and the two glands normally contain almost
exactly the same amount of adrenin. Elliott 9 found that when the gland
on one side was isolated by cutting its splanchnic supply, and then
impulses were sent along the intact nerves of the other side, either by
disturbing the animal or by artificial excitation of the nerves, the
gland to which these fibres reached invariably contained less adrenin,
often very much less, than the isolated gland. Results obtained by the
method employed by Elliott have been confirmed with remarkable
exactness in results obtained by Folin, Denis and myself, 10
using a highly sensitive color test after adding the gland extract to a
solution of phosphotungstic acid.
All these observations, with a variety of methods, and by a
respectable number of reliable investigators,
are harmonious in bringing proof that artificial stimulation
of the nerves leading to the adrenal glands will induce secretory
activity in the adrenal medulla, and that in consequence adrenin will
be increased in the blood. The fact is therefore securely established
that in the body a mechanism exists by which these
glands can be made to discharge this peculiar substance
promptly into the circulation.
THE QUESTION OF ADRENAL SECRETION IN EMOTIONAL EXCITEMENT
As we have already seen, the phenomena of a great emotional
disturbance in an animal indicate that sympathetic impulses dominate
the viscera. When, for example, a cat becomes frightened, the pupils
dilate, the activities of the stomach and intestines are inhibited, the
heart beats rapidly, the hairs of the back and tail stand erect from
one end of the animal to the other there are abundant signs of nervous
discharges along sympathetic courses. Do not the adrenal glands share
in this widespread subjugation of the viscera to sympathetic control?
This question, whether the common excitements of an animal's
life might be capable of evoking a discharge of adrenin, was taken up
by D. de la Paz and myself in 1910. We made use of the natural enmity
between two laboratory animals, the dog and the cat, to pursue pur
experiments. In these experiments the cat, fastened in a comfortable
holder (the holder already mentioned as being used in X-ray studies of
the movements of the alimentary canal), was placed near a barking dog.
Some cats when thus treated showed almost no signs of fear; others,
with scarcely a movement of defence, presented the typical picture. In
favorable cases the excitement was allowed to prevail for five or ten
minutes, and in a few cases longer. Samples of blood were taken within
a few minutes before and after the period.
THE METHOD OF SECURING BLOOD FROM NEAR THE ADRENAL VEINS
The blood was obtained from the inferior vena cava anterior
to the opening of the adrenal veins, i. e., at a point inside the body
near the level of the notch at the lower end of the sternum. To get the
blood so far from the surface without disturbing the animal was at
first a difficult problem. We found, however, that by making anesthetic
with ethyl chloride the skin directly over the femoral vein high in the
groin, the vein could
be quickly bared, cleared of connective tissue, tied, and
opened, without causing any general disturbance whatever. A long, fine,
flexible catheter (2.4 millimeters in diameter) which had previously
been coated with vaseline inside and out, to lubricate it and to delay
the clotting of blood within it, was now introduced into the opening in
the femoral vein, thence through the iliac and on into the inferior
cava to a point near the level of the sternal notch. A thread tied
around this tube where, after being inserted to the proper distance,
it disappeared into the femoral vein, marked the extent of
insertion, and permitted a later introduction to the same extent. This
slight operation a venesection, commonly practised on our ancestors
consumed only a few minutes, and as the only possibility of causing
pain was guarded against by local anesthesia, the animal
remained tranquil throughout. Occasionally it was necessary
to stroke the cat's head gently to keep her quiet on the holder, and
under such circumstances I have known her to purr during all the
preparations for obtaining the blood, and while the blood was being
taken.
The blood (3 or 4 cubic centimetres) was slowly drawn through
the catheter into a clean glass syringe. Care was taken to avoid any
marked suction such as might cause collapse of the vein near the inner
opening of the tube. As soon as the blood was secured, the catheter was
removed and the vein tied loosely, to prevent bleeding. The blood was
at once emptied into a beaker, and the fibrin whipped from it by means
of fringed
rubber tubing fitted over a glass rod. Since this
defibrinated blood was obtained while the animal was undisturbed, it
was labelled "quiet blood."
The animal was then exposed to the barking dog, as already
described, and immediately thereafter blood was again removed, from
precisely the same region as before. This sample, after being
defibrinated, was labelled "excited blood." the two samples, the
"quiet" and the "excited," both obtained in the same manner and
subsequently treated in the same manner, were now tested for their
content of adrenin.
THE METHOD OF TESTING THE BLOOD FOR ADRENIN
It was desirable to use as a test tissues to which the blood
was naturally related. As will be recalled, adrenin affects viscera
even after they have been removed from the body, just as if they were
receiving impulses via sympathetic fibres, and further, that
sympathetic fibres normally deliver impulses which cause contraction
of the internal genitals and relaxation of the stomach and
intestines. The uterus has long been employed as a test for adrenin,
the presence of which it indicates by increased contraction. That
isolated strips of the longitudinal muscle of the intestine, which are
contracting rhythmically, are characteristically inhibited by adrenin
in dilutions of 1 part in 20 millions, had been shown by Magnus in
1905. Although, previous to our investigation in 1910, this extremely
delicate reaction had not been used as a biological signal for adrenin,
it possesses noteworthy advantages over other methods. The intestine is
found in all animals and not in only half of them, as is the uterus; it
is ready for the test within a few minutes, instead of the several
hours said to be required for the best use of the uterus preparation;
11 and it responds by relaxing. This last characteristic
is especially important, for in deflbrinated blood there are,
besides adrenin, other substances capable of causing contraction of
smooth muscle, 12 and liable therefore to lead to erroneous conclusions
when a structure which responds by contracting, such as uterus or
artery, is used to prove whether adrenin is present. On the other hand,
substances producing relaxation of smooth muscle are few, and are
unusual in blood.13
We used, therefore, the strip of intestinal muscle as an
indicator. Later Hoskins 14 modified our procedure by taking, instead
of the strip, a short segment of the rabbit intestine. The segment is
not subjected to danger of injury during its preparation, and when
fresh it is almost incredibly sensitive. It may be noticeably inhibited
by adrenin, 1 part in 200 millions !
The strip, or the intestinal segment, was suspended between
minute wire pincers (serres fines) in a cylindrical chamber 8
millimeters in diameter and 5 centimeters deep. By a thread attached to
the lower serre fine the preparation was drawn into the
chamber, and was held firmly; by the upper one it was attached to the
short end of a writing lever (see Fig. 2). When not exposed to blood,
the strip was immersed in a normal solution of the blood salts
(Ringer's). The blood or the salt solution could be quickly withdrawn
from or introduced into the chamber, with out disturbing the
muscle, by means of a fine pipette passed down along the inner surface.
The chamber and its contents, the stock of Ringer's
solution, and the samples of "quiet" and "excited" blood were
all surrounded by a large volume of water kept approximately at body
temperature (37 C.). Through the blood or the salt solution in the
chamber oxygen was passed in a slow but steady stream of bubbles. Under
these circumstances the strip will live for hours, and will contract
and relax in a beautifully regular rhythm, which may be recorded
graphically by
the writing lever.
The first effect of surrounding the muscle with blood,
whether "quiet" or "excited," was to send it into a strong contraction
which might persist, sometimes with slight oscillations, for a minute
or two (see Figs. 4 and 5), After the initial shortening, the strip, if
in quiet blood soon began to contract and relax rhythmically and with
each relaxation to lengthen more, until a fairly even base line
appeared in the written record. At this
stage the addition of fresh "quiet" blood usually had no
effect, even though the strip were washed once with Einger's solution
before the second portion of the blood was added. For comparison of the
effects of "quiet" and "excited" blood on the contracting strip, the
two samples were each added to the muscle immediately after the
Ringer's solution had been removed, or they were applied to the muscle
alternately and the differences in effect then noted. The results
obtained by these methods are next to be presented.
REFERENCES
1 Jacobi: Archiv für experimentelle Pathologie und
Pharmakologie, 1891, xxix, p. 185.
2 Biedl: Archiv für die gesammte Physiologie, 1897,
Ixvii,
pp. 456, 481.
8 Dreyer: American Journal of Physiology, 1898-99, ii, p.
219.
4 Tscheboksarof?: Archiv für die gesammte Physiologie,
1910,
cxxxvii, p. 103.
5 Asher: Zeitschrift für Biologie, 1912, Iviii, p. 274.
6 Meltzer and Joseph: American Journal of Physiology, 1912,
xxix, p. xxxiv.
7 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xliv, p. 400.
8 Cannon and Lyman: American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxi, p. 377.
9 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xliv, p. 400.
10 Folin, Cannon and Denis: Journal of Biological Chemistry,
1913, xiii, p. 477.
11 Fraenkel: Archiv für experimentelle Pathologie und
Pharmakologie, 1909, Ix, p. 399.
12 See O'Connor: Archiv für die experimentelle
Pathologie und
Pharmakologie, 1912, Ixvii, p. 206.
13 Grutzner: Ergebnisse der Physiologic, 1904, iii2, p. 66;
Magnus: Loc. cit., p. 69.
14 Hoskins: Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental
Therapeutics, 1911, iii, p. 95.
CHAPTER IV
ADRENAL SECRETION IN STRONG EMOTIONS AND PAIN
If the secretion of adrenin is increased in strong emotional
states and in pain, that constitutes a fact of considerable
significance, for, as already mentioned, adrenin is capable of
producing many of the bodily changes which are characteristically
manifested in emotional and painful experiences. It is a matter of
prime importance for further discussion to determine whether the
adrenal glands are in fact roused to special activity in times of
stress.
THE EVIDENCE THAT ADRENAL SECRETION IN INCREASED IN EMOTIONAL
EXCITEMENT
That blood from the adrenal veins causes the relaxation of
intestinal muscle characteristic of adrenal extract or adrenin is shown
in Fig. 3. The muscle was originally beating in blood which contained
no demonstrable amount of adrenal secretion; this inactive blood was
replaced by blood from the adrenal veins, obtained after quick
etherization. Etherization, it will be recalled, is accompanied by a
"stage of excitement." Relaxation occurred almost immediately (at b),
Then the rhythm was renewed in the former
with blood from the vein leading away from the left kidney,
i. e., blood obtained from the same animal and under the same
conditions as the adrenal blood, but from a neighboring vein. No
relaxation occurred. By this and other similar tests the reliability of
the method was proved.
In no instance did blood from the inferior vena cava of the
quiet normal animal produce relaxation. On the other hand, blood from
the animal after emotional excitement showed more or less promptly the
typical relaxation. In Fig. 4 is
represented the record of intestinal muscle which was beating
regularly in Ringer's solution. At a the Ringer's solution was removed,
and at b "excited" blood was added; after the preliminary
shortening, which, as already stated, occurs at the first immersion in
blood, the muscle lengthened gradually into complete inhibition. At c
the "excited" blood was removed, and at d "quiet" blood was added in
its place. The muscle at once began fairly regular rhythmic beats. At e
the "quiet" blood was removed, and at f the "excited"
blood was again applied. The muscle lengthened almost
immediately into an inhibited state. In this instance the "excited"
blood was taken after the cat had been barked at for about fifteen
minutes.
The increase of effect with prolongation of the period of
excitement is shown in Fig. 5. A is the
record of contractions after the muscle was surrounded with
"quiet" blood serum. B shows the gradual inhibition which occurred when
the muscle was surrounded with defibrinated blood taken when the animal
had been excited eleven minutes. And C is the record of rapid
inhibition after fifteen minutes of excitement. In other instances the
effect was manifested merely by a lowering of the tonus of the muscle,
and a notable slowing of the beats, without, however, a total abolition
of them.
The inference that this inhibition of contraction of the
intestinal muscle is due to an increased amount of adrenal secretion in
the "excited" blood de la Paz and I justified on several grounds:
(1) The inhibition was produced by "excited" blood from the
inferior vena cava anterior to the mouths of the adrenal veins, when
blood from the femoral vein, taken at the same time, had no inhibitory
influence. Since blood from the femoral vein is typical of the cava
blood below the entrance of the kidney veins, the conclusion is
warranted that the difference of effect of the two samples of blood is
not due to any agent below the kidneys. But that blood from the kidneys
does not cause the relaxation is shown in Fig. 3.
The only other structures which could alter the blood between
the two points at which it was taken are the adrenal glands, and the
material secreted by them would produce precisely the inhibition of
contraction which was in fact produced.
(2) If in ether anesthesia the blood vessels leading to and
from the adrenal glands are first
carefully tied, and then the glands are removed, excitement
four or five hours later, before the weakness that follows
the removal has become prominent, does not alter the blood so that the
typical inhibition occurs (see Fig. 6). Thus, although the animal shows
all the characteristic signs of sympathetic stimulation, the blood, in
the absence of the adrenals, remains unchanged.
(3) As already shown, sometimes the effect produced by the
"excited" blood was prompt inhibition,
sometimes the inhibition followed only after several beats,
and sometimes a slowing and shortening of contractions, with a lower
tone, were the sole signs of the action of adrenin. All these degrees
of relaxation can be duplicated by adding to inactive blood varying
amounts of adrenin. Fig. 7 shows the effects, on a somewhat insensitive
muscle preparation, of adding adrenin, 1:1,000,000 (A), 1:2,000,000
(B), and 1:3,000,000 (C), to different samples of blood previously
without inhibitory influence. These effects of adrenin and the effects
produced by blood taken near the opening of the adrenal veins are
strikingly analogous.
(4) Emden and v. Furth 1 have reported that 0.1 gram of
suprarenin chloride disappears almost completely in two hours if added
to 200 cubic centimeters of defibrinated beef blood, and the mixture
constantly aerated at body temperature. "Excited" blood which produces
inhibition loses that power on standing in the cold for twenty-four
hours, or on being kept warm and agitated with bubbling oxygen. This
change is illustrated in
Fig. 8; the power of the "excited" blood to inhibit the
contractions of the intestinal muscle when record
A was written was destroyed after three hours of exposure to
bubbling oxygen, as shown by record B. The destruction of adrenin and
the disappearance of the effect which adrenin would produce are thus
closely parallel.
All these considerations, taken with the proof
that sympathetic impulses increase secretion of the adrenal
glands, and taken also with the evidence that, during such emotional
excitement as was employed in these experiments, signs of sympathetic
discharges appeared throughout the animal from the dilated pupil of the
eye to the standing hairs of the tail-tip, led us to the conclusions
that the characteristic action of adrenin on intestinal muscle was in
fact, in our experiments, due to secretion of the adrenal glands, and
that that secretion is increased in great emotion.
THE EVIDENCE THAT ADRENAL SECRETION IS INCREASED BY "PAINFUL"
STIMULATION
As mentioned in the first chapter, stimulation of sensory
fibres in one of the larger nerve trunks is known to result in such,
nervous discharges along sympathetic paths as to produce marked
inhibition of digestive processes. Other manifestations of sympathetic
innervations e. g., contraction of arterioles, dilation of pupils,
erection of hairs are also demonstrable. And since the adrenal glands
are stimulated to activity by sympathetic impulses, it was possible
that they would be affected as are other structures supplied with
sympathetic fibres, arid that they would secrete in greater abundance
when sensory nerves were irritated.
The testing of this possibility was undertaken by Hoskins and
myself in 1911. Since bodily changes from "painful" stimulation can in
large degree be produced in an anesthetized animal, without, however,
an experience of pain by the animal, it was possible to make the test
quite simply. The sensory stimulus was a rapidly interrupted induced
current applied to the sciatic nerve. The current was increased in
strength as time passed, and thus the intensity of the effect,
indicated by continuous dilation of the pupils, was maintained. There
was no doubt that such stimulation would have caused very severe pain
if the animal had not been anesthetized. Indeed, the stimulus used was
probably much stronger than would be necessary to obtain
a positive result in the absence of the anesthetic
(urethane), which markedly lessens the irritability of visceral nerve
fibres. 2 In different instances the stimulation lasted from three to
six minutes. Throughout the period there was markedly increased
rapidity and depth of breathing.
As Fig. 9 shows, the normal blood, removed
from the vena cava before stimulation, caused no inhibition
of the beating segment, whereas that removed afterwards produced a deep
relaxation. Hoskins and I showed that the increased respiration which
accompanies "painful" stimulation does not augment adrenal activity. We
concluded, therefore, that when a sensory trunk is strongly excited the
adrenal glands are reflexly stimulated, and that they pour into the
blood stream an increased amount of adrenin.
CONFIRMATION OF OUR RESULTS BY OTHER OBSERVERS
The foregoing experiments and conclusions were reported in
1911. In 1912. Anrep 3 found that a denervated limb at first expands
but later quickly contracts on sensory stimulation. The phase of
contraction disappeared if the adrenal glands were removed. Since the
limb was denervated, the only agency which could have caused
contraction in the presence of increased blood pressure must have been
brought by the blood stream. And, since the phenomenon disappeared on
exclusion of the adrenals, the conclusion was drawn that adrenal
secretions, poured out in consequence of reflex stimulation,
produced the observed vaso-constriction.
The following year, Hitchings, Sloan and Austin, 4 using our
method, found that after great fear and rage had been induced in a cat,
the adrenin reaction was clearly demonstrated. The reaction did not
occur, however, if the splanchnic nerves had been previously severed.
In 1913, also, Levy 4 reported that sciatic stimulation occasioned
irregularity of the denervated heart, an effect likewise seen when a
splanchnic nerve was stimulated, but lacking after adrenal extirpation
in the stimulated side. Again the conclusion was drawn
that the effect was due to adrenin discharged reflexly.
Similar evidence was reported in 1917 by Florovsky,
6 with reference to the denervated salivary gland.
Furthermore, Redfield noted (1917) 7 that nervous excitement causes a
contraction of the melanophores in the denervated skin of the horned
toad, a reaction which is absent after removal of the adrenal glands.
Recently I have myself 8 completely confirmed all our earlier work, by
using the denervated heart as an indicator. The only opposition to all
these positive results is that offered by Stewart and Rogoff, who
failed to demonstrate any effects of sensory stimulation in adrenal
activity. Since they used a peculiar method, however, known to disturb
profoundly the region innervated by the splanchnic nerves, their
negative results cannot be regarded as offsetting the positive findings
of all other observers.
The logic of all these experiments may be briefly summed up.
That the adrenal glands are subject to splanchnic influence has been
demonstrated anatomically and by the physiological effects of their
secretion after artificial stimulation of the splanchnic nerves.
Impulses are normally sent along these nerves, in the natural
conditions of life, when animals become greatly excited, as in fear and
rage and pain. There is every probability, therefore, that these glands
are stimulated to extra secretion at such times. Both by an exceedingly
delicate biological test (intestinal muscle) and by an examination of
the glands themselves,
clear evidence has been secured that in pain and deep emotion
the glands do, in fact, pour out an excess of adrenin into the
circulating blood.
Here, then, is a remarkable group of phenomena a pair of
glands stimulated to activity in times of strong excitement and by such
nerve impulses as themselves produce at such times profound changes in
the viscera; and a secretion given forth into the blood stream by these
glands, which is capable of inducing by itself, or of augmenting, the
nervous influences which induce the very changes in the viscera which
accompany suffering and the major emotions. What may be the
significance of these changes, occurring when conditions of pain and
great excitement experiences common to animals of most diverse types
and
probably known to their ancestors for ages past lay hold of
the bodily functions and determine
the instinctive responses?
Certain remarkable effects of injecting adrenin into the
blood have for many years been more or less well recognized. For
example, when injected it causes liberation of sugar from the liver
into the blood stream. It relaxes the smooth muscle of the bronchioles.
Some old experiments indicated that it acts as an antidote for muscular
fatigue. It alters the distribution of the blood in the body, driving
it from the abdominal viscera
into the heart, lungs, central nervous system and limbs. And
there was some evidence that it renders more rapid the coagulation of
the blood.
There may be other activities of adrenin not yet discovered
it may co-operate with the products of other glands of internal
secretion. And other glands of internal secretion may be stimulated by
sympathetic impulses. But we were not concerned with these
possibilities. We wished to know whether the adrenin poured out in pain
and emotional excitement produced or helped to produce the same effects
that follow the injection of adrenin. Our later researches were
directed towards securing answers to this question.
REFERENCES
1 Embden and v. Furth: Hofmeister's Beiträge zur
chemischen
Physiologie und Pathologie, 19O4, iv, p. 423.
2 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1005, xxxii, p. 448.
3 Anrep: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xiv, p. 307.
4 Hitchings, Sloan and Austin: Cleveland Medical Journal,
1913, xii, p. 686.
5 Levy: Heart, 1913, iv, p. 342.
6 Florovsky: Bulletin de l'Académie Impériale
des Sciences,
Petrograd, 1917, ix, p. 119.
7 Redfield: Journal of Experimental Zoology, 1918, xxvi, p.
295.
s Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1919, 1, p. 399.
9 Stewart and Rogoff: Journal of Experimental Medicine, 1917,
xxvi, p. 637.
CHAPTER V
THE INCREASE OF BLOOD SUGAR IN PAIN AND GREAT EMOTION
Sugar is the form in which carbohydrate material is
transported in organisms; starch is the storage form. In the bodies of
animals that have been well fed the liver contains an abundance of
glycogen or "animal starch," which may be called upon in times of need.
At such times the glycogen is changed, and set free in the blood as
sugar. Ordinarily there is a small percentage of sugar in the blood
from 0.06 to 0.1 per cent. When only this small amount is present the
kidneys are capable of preventing its escape in any noteworthy
amount. If the percentage rises to the neighborhood of
0.2-0.3 per cent, however, the sugar passes the obstacle set up by the
kidneys, and is readily demonstrable in the urine by ordinary tests.
The condition of "glycosuria," therefore, may properly be considered,
in certain circumstances, as evidence of increased sugar in the blood.
The injection of adrenin can liberate sugar from the liver to such an
extent that glycosuria results. Does the adrenal secretion discharged
in pain and strong emotional excitement play a role in producing
glycosuria under such conditions?
In clinical literature scattered suggestions are to be found
that conditions giving rise to emotional states may be the occasion
also of more or less permanent glycosuria. Great grief and prolonged
anxiety during a momentous crisis have been regarded as causes of
individual instances of diabetes, and anger or fright has been followed
by an increase in the sugar excreted by persons who already have the
disease. Kleen 1 cites
the instance of a German officer whose diabetes and whose
Iron Cross for valor both came from a stressful experience in the
Franco-Prussian War. The onset of the disease in a man directly after
his wife was discovered in adultery is described by Naunyn; 2 and this
author also mentions two cases in his own practice one started during
the bombardment of Strassburg (1870), the other started a few days
after a companion had shot himself. In cases of mental disease, also,
states of depression have been described accompanied by sugar in the
urine. Schultze 3 has reported that in these cases the amount of
glycosuria is dependent on the degree of depression, and that the
greatest excretion of sugar occurs in the fearpsychoses. Raimann 4 has
reported that in both melancholia and mania the assimilation limit of
sugar may be lowered. Similar results in the insane have recently been
presented by Mita, 5 and by Folin and Denis. 6 The latter investigators
found glycosuria in 12 per cent of 192 insane patients, most
of whom suffered from depression, apprehension, or excitement. And
Arndt 7 has observed glycosuria appearing and disappearing as alcoholic
delirium appeared and disappeared in his patients.
Although clinical evidence thus indicates an emotional origin
of some cases of diabetes and glycosuria, the intricacies of existence
and the complications of disease in human beings throw some doubt on
the value of that evidence. Both Naunyn 8 and Hirschfeld, although
mentioning instances of diabetes apparently due to an emotional
experience, urge a skeptical attitude toward such statements. It is
desirable, therefore,
that the question of an emotional glycosuria be tested under
simpler and more controllable conditions.
"Emotional glycosuria" in experimental animals has indeed
been referred to by Waterman and Smit 9 and more recently by Henderson
and Underhill.10 Both these references, however, are based on the work
of Bohm and Hoffmann,11 reported in 1878.
GLYCOSURIA FROM PAIN
Bohm and Hoffmann found that cats, when bound to an operating
board, a tube inserted into the trachea (without anesthesia), and in
some instances a catheter inserted into the urethra through an opening
above the pubis, had in about half an hour an abundance of sugar in the
urine. In three determinations sugar in the blood proved slightly above
"normal" so long as sugar was appearing in the urine, but returned to
"normal"
as the glycosuria disappeared. Since they were able to
produce the phenomenon by simply binding animals to the holder, they
called it "Fesselungsdiabetes."
As possible causes of this glycosuria in bound animals, they
considered opening the trachea, cooling, and pain. The first two they
readily eliminated, and still they found sugar excreted. Pain they
could not obviate, and since, without binding the animals, they caused
glycosuria by merely stimulating the sciatic nerves, they concluded
that painful confinement was itself a sufficient cause. Other factors,
however, such as cooling
and circulatory disturbances, probably cooperated with pain,
they believed, to produce the result. Their observations on cats have
been proved true also of rabbits; 12 and recently it has been shown
that an operation involving some pain increases blood sugar in dogs. 13
Temporary glycosuria has likewise been noted in association with
intense pain in human beings.
Inasmuch as Bohm and Hoffmann did not mention the emotional
element in discussing their results, and inasmuch as they admitted that
they could not obviate from their experimental procedure pain, which
they themselves proved was effective in causing glycosuria, designating
what they called "Fesselungsdiabetes" as "emotional glycosuria" is not
justified.
EMOTIONAL GLYCOSURIA
The discovery that during strong emotion adrenal secretion is
increased, and the fact that injection of adrenin gives rise to
glycosuria, suggested that glycosuria might be called forth by
emotional excitement, and then that even without the painful element of
Bohm and Hoffmann's experiments, sugar might be found in the urine. The
testing of this possibility was undertaken by A. T. Shohl, W. S. Wright
and myself in 1911.
Our first procedure was a repetition of Bohm and Hoffmann's
experiments, freed from the factor of pain. The animals (cats) were
bound to a comfortable holder, which left the head unfastened. This
holder I had used hundreds of times in X-ray studies of digestion, with
many different animals, without causing any signs
of even so much as uneasiness. Just as in observations on the
movements of the alimentary canal, however, so here, the animals
reacted differently to the experience of being confined. Young males
usually became quite frantic, and with eyes wide, pupils dilated, pulse
accelerated, hairs of the tail more or less erect, they struggled,
snarling and growling, to free themselves. Females, on the contrary,
especially if elderly, were as a rule much more calm, and resignedly
accepted the novel situation.
According to differences in reaction the animals were left in
the holder for periods varying in length from thirty minutes to five
hours. In order to insure prompt urination, considerable quantities of
water were given by stomach tube at the beginning of the experiment and
in some cases again later. Arrangements were made for draining the
urine promptly, when the animal was on the holder or when afterwards in
a metal metabolism cage, into a glass receiver containing a few drops
of chloroform to prevent fermentation. The diet in all cases consisted
of customary raw meat and milk. In every instance the urine was proved
free from sugar before the animal was excited.
In our series of observations twelve cats were used, and in
every one a well-marked glycosuria was developed. The shortest periods
of confinement to the holder which were effective were thirty and forty
minutes; the longest we employed, five hours. The average time required
to bring about a glycosuria was less than an hour and a half; the
average in seven of the twelve cases was less than forty minutes. In
all cases no sugar was found in the urine passed on the day after the
excitement.
The promptness with which the glycosuria developed was
directly related to the emotional state of the animal. Sugar was found
early in animals which early showed signs of being frightened or in a
rage, and much later in animals which took the experience more calmly.
As cooling may result in increased sugar in the blood, and
consequent glycosuria, the rectal temperature
was observed from time to time, and it was found to vary so
slightly that in these experiments it was a wholly negligible factor.
In one cat the rectal temperature fell to 36 C. while the animal was
bound and placed in a cold room (about 2 C.) for fifty minutes, but no
sugar appeared in the urine.
Further evidence that the appearance of sugar in the urine
may arise purely from emotional excitement was obtained from three cats
which gave negative results when bound in the holder for varying
periods up to four hours. It was noteworthy that these animals remained
calm and passive in their confinement. When, however, they were placed,
separately, in a small wire cage, and were barked at by an energetic
little dog, that jumped at them and made signs of attack, the cats
became much excited, they showed their teeth,
humped their backs, and growled defiance. This sham fight was
permitted to continue for a half hour in each of the three cases. In
each case the animal, which after four hours of bondage had exhibited
no glycosuria, now had sugar in the urine.
Pain, cooling, and bondage were not factors in these
experiments. The animal was either frightened or enraged by the barking
dog, and that excitement was attended by glycosuria.
The sugar excreted in the twenty-four hours which included
the period of excitement was determined by the Bertrand method.14 It
ranged from 0.024 gram to 1.93 grams, or from 0.008 gram to 0.62 gram
per kilo body weight, for the twenty-four hours' quantity.
The presence of sugar in the urine may be used as an
indication of increased sugar in the blood, for unless injury has been
done to the cells of the kidneys, they do not permit sugar to escape
until the percentage in the blood has risen to a considerable degree.
Thus, though testing the urine reveals the instances of a high content
of blood sugar, it does not show the fine variations that appear when
the blood itself is examined.
Recently Scott 15 has concluded a thorough in vestigation of
the variations of blood sugar in cats, and has found that merely
incidental conditions, producing even mild excitement, as indicated by
crying or otherwise, result in a noticeable rise in the amount. Indeed,
so sensitive is the sugar-liberating mechanism that all the early
determinations of the "normal" content of sugar in blood which has been
drawn from an artery or vein in the absence of anesthesia, are of very
doubtful value. Certainly when care is taken to obtain
blood suddenly from a tranquil animal, the percentage (0.069,
Scott; 0.088, Pavy) is much less than when the blood is drawn without
anesthesia (0.15, Bohm and Hoffmann), or after light narcosis (0.282,
Eona and Takahashi 16 ).
Our observations on cats have since been found valid for
rabbits. Rolly and Oppermann, Jacobsen, and Hirsch and Reinbach 17 have
recently recorded that the mere handling of a rabbit preparatory to
operating on it will increase the percentage of blood sugar (in some
cases from 0.10 to 0.23 and 0.27 per cent). Dogs are said to be much
less likely to be disturbed by the nature of their surroundings than
are rabbits and cats.
Nevertheless, pain and excitement are such fundamental
experiences in animals that without much
doubt the same mechanism is operative in all when these
experiences occur. Probably, just as the
digestion of dogs is disturbed by strong emotion, the blood
sugar likewise is increased, for sympathetic
impulses occasion both changes.* Gib has given an account of
a bitch that became much agitated when shut up, and after such enforced
seclusion, but never otherwise, she excreted small quantities of sugar
in the urine.18
The results noted in these lower animals have been confirmed
in human beings. One of my former students, W. G. Smillie, found that
four of nine medical students, all normally without sugar in their
urine, had glycosuria after a hard examination, and only one of the
nine had glycosuria after an easier examination. The tests, which were
positive with Fehling's solution, Nylander's reagent, and also with
phenyl-hydrazine, were
made on the first urine passed after the examination.
Furthermore, C. H. Fiske and I examined the urine of twenty-five
members of the Harvard University football squad immediately after the
final and most exciting contest of the season of 1913, and found sugar
in twelve cases. Five of these positive cases were among substitutes
not called upon to enter the game. The only excited spectator of the
Harvard
* Since the foregoing sentences were written Hirsch
and
Eeinbach have reported (Zeitschrift für physiologische Chemie,
1914,
xci, p. 292) a "psychic hyperglycemia" in dogs, that resulted from
fastening the animals to a table. The blood sugar rose in one instance
from 0.11 to 0.14 per cent, and in another from 0.09 to 0.16 per cent.
victory whose urine was examined also had a marked
glycosuria, which on the following day had disappeared. Other tests
made on students before and after important scholastic examinations
have been published by Folin, Denis and Smillie. 19 Of thirtyfour
second-year medical students tested, one had sugar before the
examination as well as afterwards. Of the remaining thirty-three, six,
or 18 per cent, had small but unmistakable traces of sugar in the urine
passed directly following the ordeal. A similar study was made on
second-year students at a women's college. Of thirty-six students who
had no sugar in the urine on the day
before, six, or 17 per cent, eliminated sugar with the urine
passed immediately after the examination. From the foregoing results it
is reasonable to conclude that just as in the cat, dog, and rabbit, so
also in man, emotional excitement produces temporary increase of blood
sugars
THE ROLE OP THE ADRENAL GLANDS IN EMOTIONAL GLYCOSURIA
Since artificial stimulation of the splanchnic nerves
produces glycosuria, 20 and since major emotions, such as rage and
fright, are attended by nervous discharges along splanchnic pathways,
glycosuria as an accompaniment of emotional excitement would naturally
be expected to occur. To what extent the adrenal glands which, as
already mentioned, are stimulated to increased secretion by excitement,
might play a part in this process, has been in dispute. Removal of
these glands or cutting of the nerve fibres supplying
them, according to some observers, 21 prevents glycosuria
after puncture of the fourth ventricle of the brain (the "sugar
puncture," which typically induces glycosuria) and also after
stimulation of the splanchnics. 22 On the other hand, Wertheimer and
Battez 23 have stated that removal of the glands does not abolish the
effects of sugar puncture in the cat. It was questionable, therefore,
whether removal of the adrenal glands would affect emotional
glycosuria.
Evidence on this point I secured with Shohl and Wright in
observations on three animals in which the adrenals were removed
aseptically under ether. The animals selected had all become quickly
excited on being bound to the holder, and had manifested glycosuria
after about an hour of confinement. In the operation, to avoid
discharge of adrenin by handling, the adrenal veins were first tied,
and then the glands freed from their attachments and removed as quickly
and with as little manipulation as possible. In one cat the entire
operation was finished in twenty minutes. In two of the cats a small
catheter was introduced into the urethra through an incision, so that
the bladder could be emptied at any time.
In all three cases urine that was free from sugar was
obtained soon after the operation. Although the animals deprived of
their adrenals manifested a general lessening of muscular tone, they
still displayed much of their former rage or excitement when bound.
Indeed, one was more excited after removal of the adrenals than before.
That the animals might not be excessively cooled they were kept warm
with coverings or an electric
heating pad. Although they were now bound for periods from
two to three times as long as the periods required formerly to cause
glycosuria, no trace of sugar was found in the urine in any instance.
The evidence thus secured tends, therefore, to support the view that
the adrenal glands perform an important contributory role in the
glycosuria resulting from splanchnic stimulation.
Possibly the emotional element is in part accountable for the
glycosuria observed after painful stimulation, but conditions causing
pain alone will reasonably explain it. As we have already seen, strong
stimulation of sensory fibres causes the discharge of impulses along
the splanchnic nerves, and incidentally calls forth an increased
secretion of the adrenal glands. In glycosuria resulting from painful
stimulation, as well as in emotional glycosuria, the adrenal glands may
be essential factors.
Later the evidence will be given that sugar is the optimum
source of muscular energy. In passing, we may note that the liberation
of sugar at a time when great muscular exertion is likely to be
demanded of the organism may be interpreted as a highly interesting
instance of biological adaptation.
REFERENCES
1 Kleen: On Diabetes Mellitus and Glycosuria, Philadelphia,
1900, pp. 22, 37-39.
2 Naunyn: Der Diabetes Mellitus, Vienna, 1898, p. 72.
3 Schultze: Verhandlungen der Gesellschaft deutscher
Naturforscher und Aerzte, Cologne, 1908, ii, p. 358.
4 Raimann: Zeitschrift für Heilkunde, 1902, xxiii,
Abtheilung
iii, pp. 14, 19.
5 Mita: Monatshefte für Psychiatrie und Neurologie,
1912,
xxxii, p. 159.
6 Folin, Denis and Smillie: Journal of Biological Chemistry,
1914, xvii, p. 519.
7 Arndt: Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde, 1897, x. p.
436.
8 Naunyn: Loc. cit. f p. 73; Hirschfeld: Die Zuckerkrankheit,
Leipzig, 1902, p. 45.
9 Waterman and Smit: Archiv für die gesammte
Physiologie,
1908, cxxiv, p. 205.
10 Henderson and Underhill: American Journal of Physiology,
1911, xxviii, p. 276.
11 Bohm and Hoffmann: Archiv für experimentelle
Pathologie
und Pharmakologie, 1878, viii, p. 295.
12 Eckhard: Zeitschrift für Biologie, 1903, xliv, p.
408.
18 Loewy and Rosenberg: Biochemische Zeitschrift, 1913, Ivi,
p. 114.
14 See Abderhalden: Handbuch der biochemischen
Arbeitsmethoden, Berlin, 1910, ii, p. 181.
15 Scott: American Journal of Physiology, 1914, xxxiv, p.
283.
16 Cited by Scott: Loc. cit., p. 296.
17 Roily and Oppermann: Biochemische Zeitschrift, 1913, xlix,
p. 201. Jacobsen: Ibid., 1913, li, p. 449. Hirsch and Reinbach:
Zeitschrift für physiologische Chemie, 1913, Ixxxvii, p. 122.
18 Cited by Kleen: Loc. cit., p. 37.
19 Folin, Denis and Smillie: Loc. cit.,, p. 520.
20 See Macleod: American Journal of Physiology, 1907, xix, p.
405, also for other references to literature.
21 See Meyer: Comptes rend us de la Societé de
Biologie,
1906, Iviii, p. 1123; Nishi: Archiv für experimentelle Pathologie
und
Pharmakologie, 1909, Ixi, p. 416.
22 Gautrelct and Thomas: Comptes rcndus de la Societé
de
Biologie, 1909, ixvii, p. 233; and Macleod: Proceedings of the Society
for Experimental Biology and Medicine, 1911, viii, p. 110 (true for
left adrenal and left splanchnic).
23 Wertheimer and Battez: Archives Internationales de
Physiologie, 1910, ix, p. 392.
CHAPTER VI
IMPROVED CONTRACTION OF FATIGUED MUSCLE AFTER SPLANCHNIC
STIMULATION OF
THE ADRENAL GLAND
In the older literature on the adrenal glands the deleterious
effect of their absence, or the beneficial effect of injected extracts,
on the contraction of skeletal muscle was not infrequently noted. As
evidence accumulated, however, tending to prove an important relation
between the extract of the adrenal medulla (adrenin) and the
sympathetic nervous system, the relations with the efficiency of
skeletal muscle began to receive less consideration.
The muscular weakness of persons suffering from diseased
adrenals (Addison's disease) was well recognized before experimental
work on the glands was begun. Experiments on rabbits were reported in
1892 by Albanese,1 who showed that muscles which were stimulated after
removal of the glands were much more exhausted than when stimulated the
same length of time in the same animal before the removal. Similarly
Boinet 2 reported, in 1895, that rats recently deprived of their
adrenals were much more quickly exhausted
in a revolving cage than were normal animals.
That extract of the adrenal glands has the power of
increasing and prolonging the contraction of normal resting skeletal
muscle, was noted by Oliver and Schaefer, 3 in their classic original
study of the action of adrenal substance. But a recent examination of
this effect by Takayasu, 4 who employed adrenin alone, has failed to
confirm the earlier observations. It should be understood that these
observations, however, were made on resting and not on fatigued muscle.
On fatigued muscle a beneficial effect of adrenal extract, even when
applied to the solution in which the isolated muscle was contracting,
was claimed by Dessy and Grandis, 5 who studied the phenomenon in a
salamander.* Further evidence leading to the same conclusion was
offered in a discriminating
* These earlier investigations, in which an extract of
the
entire gland was used, made no distinction between the action of the
medulla and that of the cortex. It may be that the weakness following
removal or disease of the adrenals is due to absence of the cortex (see
Hoskins and Wheelon: American Journal of Physiology, 1914, xxxiv, p.
184). Such a possible effect, however, should not be confused with the
demonstrable influence of injected adrenin (derived from the adrenal
medulla alone) and the similar effects from adrenal
secretion caused by splanchnic stimulation.
paper by Panella. 6 He found that in coldblooded animals the
active principle of the adrenal medulla notably reinforced skeletal
muscle, prolonging its ability to do work, and improving its
contraction when fatigued. In warmblooded animals the same effects were
observed, but only after certain experimental procedures,
such as anesthesia and section of the bulb, had changed them
to a condition resembling the coldblooded.
The foregoing evidence indicates that removal of the adrenals
has a debilitating effect on muscular power, and that injection of
extracts of the glands has an invigorating effect. It seemed possible,
therefore, that increased secretion of the adrenal glands, whether from
direct stimulation of the splanchnic nerves or as a reflex result of
pain or the major emotions, might act as a dynamogenic factor in the
performance of muscular work. With this possibility in mind L. B. Nice
and I 7 first concerned ourselves in a research
which we conducted in 1912.
The general plan of the investigation consisted primarily in
observing the effect of stimulating the splanchnic nerves, isolated
from the spinal cord, on the contraction of a muscle whose nerve, also
isolated from the spinal cord, was rhythmically and uniformly excited
with break induction shocks. When a muscle is thus stimulated it at
first responds by strong contractions, but as time passes the
contractions become weaker, the degree of shortening of the muscle
becomes less, and in this state of lessened efficiency it may continue
for a long period to do work. The tired muscle which is showing
continuously and evenly its inability to respond as it did at first, is
said to have reached the "fatigue level." This level serves as an
excellent basis for testing influences that may have a beneficial
effect on muscular performance, for the benefit is at once manifested
in greater contraction.
In the experimental arrangement which we used, only a
connection through the circulating blood existed between the splanchnic
region and the muscle all nervous relations were severed. Any change in
muscular ability, therefore, occurring when the splanchnic nerve is
stimulated, must be due to an alteration in the quantity or quality of
the blood supplied to the laboring muscle.
Cats were used for most experiments, but results obtained
with cats were confirmed on rabbits and dogs. To produce anesthesia in
the cats and rabbits, and at the same time to avoid the fluctuating
effects of ether, urethane (2 grams per kilo body-weight) was given by
a stomach tube. The animals were fastened back downward, over an
electric warming pad, to an animal holder. Care was taken to maintain
the body temperature at its normal level throughout each experiment.
THE NERVE-MUSCLE PREPARATION
The muscle selected to be fatigued was usually the extensor
of the right hind foot (the tibialis anticus), though at
times the common extensor muscle of the digits of the same foot was
employed. The anterior tibial nerve which supplies these muscles was
bared for about two centimeters, severed toward the body, and set in
shielded electrodes, around which the skin was fastened by spring
clips. Thus the nerve could be protected,
kept moist, and stimulated without stimulation of neighboring
structures. By a small slit in the skin the tendon of the muscle was
uncovered, and after a strong thread was tied tightly about it, it was
separated from its insertion. A nerve-muscle preparation was thereby
made which was still connected with its proper blood supply. The
preparation was fixed firmly to the animal holder by thongs looped
around the hock and the foot, i. e., on either side of the slit through
which the tendon emerged.
The thread tied to the tendon was passed over a pulley and
down to a pivoted steel bar which bore a writing point. Both the pulley
and this steel writing lever were supported in a rigid tripod. In the
earliest experiments the contracting muscle was made to lift weights
(125 to 175 grams); in all the later observations, however,
the muscle pulled against a spring attached below the steel
bar. The tension of the spring as the muscle began to lift the lever
away from the support was, in most of the experiments, 110 grams, with
an increase of 10 grams as the writing point was raised 4.5
millimeters. The magnification of the lever was 3.8.
The stimuli delivered to the anterior tibial nerve were, in
most experiments, single break shocks of a value barely maximal when
applied to the fresh preparation. The rate of stimulation varied
between 60 and 300 per minute, but was uniform in any single
observation. A rate which was found generally serviceable was 180 per
minute.
Since the anterior tibial nerve contains fibres affecting
blood-vessels, as well as fibres causing contraction of skeletal
muscle, the possibility had to be considered that stimuli applied to it
might disturb the blood supply of the region. Constriction of the blood
vessels would be likely to produce the most serious disturbance, by
lessening the blood flow to the muscle. The observations of Bowditch
and Warren,8 that vasodilator rather
than vasoconstrictor effects are produced by single induction
shocks repeated at intervals of not more than five per second,
reassured us as to the danger of diminishing the blood supply, for the
rate of stimulation in our experiments never exceeded five per second
and was usually two or three. Furthermore, in using these different
rates we have never noted any result which could reasonably be
attributed to a diminished circulation.
THE SPLANCHNIC PREPARATION
The splanchnic nerves were stimulated in various ways. At
first only the left splanchnics in the abdomen were prepared. The
nerves, separated from the spinal cord, were placed upon shielded
electrodes. The form of electrodes which was found most satisfactory
was that illustrated
in Fig. 10. The instrument was made of a round rod of hard
wood, bevelled to a point at one end, and grooved on the two sides.
Into the grooves were pressed insulated wires ending in platinum hooks,
which projected beyond the bevelled surface. Around the rod was placed
an insulating rubber tube which was cut out so as to leave the hooks
uncovered when the tube was slipped downward.
In applying the electrodes the left splanchnic nerves were
first freed from their surroundings and tightly ligatured as close as
possible to their origin. By means of strong compression the
conductivity of the nerves was destroyed central to the ligature. The
electrodes were now fixed in place by thrusting the sharp end of the
wooden rod into the muscles of the back. This was so done as to bring
the platinum hooks a few millimeters above the nerves. With a small
seeker the nerves were next gently lifted over the hooks, and then the
rubber tube was slipped downward until it came in contact with the body
wall. Absorbent cotton was packed about the lower end of the
electrodes, to take up any fluid that might appear; and finally the
belly wall was closed with spring clips. The rubber tube served to keep
the platinum hooks from contact with the muscles of the back and the
movable viscera, while still permitting access to the nerves which were
to be
stimulated. This stimulating apparatus could be quickly
applied, and, once in -place, needed no further attention. In some of
the experiments both splanchnic nerves were stimulated in the thorax.
The rubber-covered electrode proved quite as serviceable there as in
the abdomen.
The current delivered to the splanchnic nerves was a rapidly
interrupted induced current of such strength that no effects of
spreading were noticeable. That splanchnic stimulation causes secretion
of the adrenal glands has been proved in many different ways which'
have already been described (see p. 41).
THE EFFECTS OF SPLANCHNIC STIMULATION ON THE CONTRACTION OF
FATIGUED MUSCLE
When skeletal muscle is repeatedly stimulated by a long
series of rapidly recurring electric shocks, its strong contractions
gradually grow weaker until a fairly constant condition is reached. The
record then has an even top the muscle has reached the "fatigue level."
The effect of splanchnic stimulation was tried when the muscle had been
fatigued to this stage. The effect which was often obtained by
stimulating the left splanchnic nerves is shown in Fig. 11. In this
instance the muscle while relaxed supported no weight, and
while contracting lifted a weight of 125 grams. The rate of
stimulation was 80 per minute.
The muscle record shows a brief initial rise from the fatigue
level, followed by a drop, and that in turn by another, prolonged rise.
The maximum height of the record is 13.5 millimeters, an increase of 6
millimeters over the height recorded before splanchnic stimulation.
Thus the muscle was performing for a short period 80 per cent more work
than before splanchnic stimulation, and for a considerably longer
period exhibited an intermediate betterment of its efficiency.
THE FIRST RISE IN THE MUSCLE RECORD
The brief first elevation in the muscle record when
registered simultaneously with arterial blood pressure is observed to
occur at the same time
with the sharp initial rise in the blood-pressure curve (see
Fig. 12). The first sharp rise in blood pressure is due to contraction
of the vessels in the area of distribution of the splanchnic nerves,
for it does not appear if the alimentary canal is removed, or if the
celiac axis and the superior and inferior mesenteric arteries are
ligated. The betterment of the muscular contraction is probably due
directly to the better blood supply resulting -from the increased
pressure, for if the adrenal veins are clipped and the splanchnic
nerves are
stimulated, the blood pressure rises as before and at the
same time there may be registered a higher
contraction of the muscle.
THE PROLONGED RISE IN THE MUSCLE RECORD
As Fig. 12 shows, the initial quick uplift in the
blood-pressure record is quickly checked by a drop. This rapid drop
does not appear when the adrenal veins are obstructed. A similar
difference in blood-pressure records has been noted before and after
excision of the adrenal glands. As Elliott, 9 and as Lyman and I 10
have shown, this sharp drop after the first rise, and also the
subsequent elevation of blood pressure, are the consequences of
liberation of adrenal secretion into the circulation. Fig. 12
demonstrates that the prolonged
rise of the muscle record begins soon after this
characteristic drop in blood pressure.
If after clips have been placed on the adrenal veins so that
no blood passes from them, the splanchnic nerves are stimulated, and
later the clips are removed, a slight but distinct improvement in the
muscular contraction occurs. As in the experiments of Young and
Lehmann, 11 in which the adrenal veins were tied for a time and
then released, the release of the blood which had been pent
in these veins was quickly followed by a rise of blood pressure. The
volume of blood thus restored to circulation was too slight to account
for the rise of pressure. In conjunction with the evidence that
splanchnic stimulation calls forth adrenal secretion, the rise may
reasonably be attributed to that secretion. The fact should be noted,
however, that in this instance the prolonged improvement in muscular
contraction did not appear until the adrenal secretion had been
admitted
to the general circulation.
Many variations in the improvement of activity in fatigued
muscle after splanchnic stimulation were noted in the course of our
investigation. The improvement varied in degree, as indicated by
increased height of the record. In some instances the height of
contraction was doubled a betterment by 100 per cent; in other
instances the contraction after splanchnic stimulation was only a small
fraction higher than that preceding the stimulation; and in still other
instances there was no betterment whatever. Never, in our experience,
were the augmented contractions equal to the original strong
contractions of the fresh muscle. The improvement also varied in degree
as indicated by persistence of effect. In some instances the muscle
returned to its former working level within four or five minutes after
splanchnic stimulation ceased (see Fig. 11); and in other cases the
muscle continued working with greater efficiency for fifteen or twenty
minutes after the stimulation.
THE TWO FACTORS: ARTERIAL PRESSURE AND ADRENAL SECRETION
The evidence just presented has shown that splanchnic
stimulation improves the contraction of fatigued muscle. Splanchnic
stimulation, however, has two effects it increases general arterial
pressure and it also causes a discharge of adrenin from the adrenal
glands. The questions now arise Does splanchnic stimulation produce the
improvement in muscular contraction by increasing the arterial blood
pressure and thereby flushing the laboring muscles with fresh blood? Or
does the adrenin liberated by splanchnic stimulation act
itself, specifically, to improve the muscular contraction? Or
may the two factors cooperate? These questions will be dealt with in
the next two chapters.
REFERENCES
1 Albanese: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1892, xvii, p.
243.
2 Boinet: Comptes rendus, Societe de Biologie, 1895, xlvii,
pp. 273, 498.
3 Oliver and Schafer: Journal of Physiology, 1895, xviii, p.
263. See also Radwanska, Anzeiger der Akademie, Krakau, 1910, pp.
728-736. Reviewed in Zentralblatt für Biochemie und Biophysik,
1911,
xi, p. 467.
4 Takayasu: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology,
1916, Ix, p. 347.
5 Dessy and Grandis: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1904,
xli, p. 231.
6 Panella: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1907, xlviii, p.
462.
7 Cannon and Nice: American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxii, p. 44.
8 Bowditch and Warren: Journal of Physiology, 1886, vii, p.
438.
9 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xliv, p. 403.
10 Cannon and Lyman: American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxi, p. 376.
11 Young and Lehmann: Journal of Physiology, 1908, xxxvii, p.
liv.
CHAPTER VII
THE EFFECTS ON CONTRACTION OF FATIGUED MUSCLE OF VARYING THE
ARTERIAL
BLOOD PRESSURE
That great excitement is accompanied by sympathetic
innervations which increase the contraction of the small arteries,
render unusually forcible the heart beat, and consequently raise
arterial pressure, has already been pointed out (see p. 26). Indeed,
the counsel to avoid circumstances likely to lead to such excitement,
which is given to persons with hardened arteries or with weak hearts,
is based on the liability of serious consequences, either in the heart
or in the vessels, that might arise from an emotional increase of
pressure in
these pathological conditions. That great muscular effort
also is accompanied by heightened arterial pressure is equally well
known, and is avoided by persons likely to be injured by it. Both in
excitement and in strong exertion the blood is forced in large degree
from the capacious vessels of the abdomen into other parts of the body.
In excitement the abdominal arteries and veins are contracted by
impulses from the splanchnic nerves.
In violent effort the diaphragm and the muscles of the belly
wall are voluntarily and antagonistically contracted in order to
stiffen the trunk as a support for the arms; and the increased
abdominal pressure which results forces blood out of that region and
does not permit reaccumulation. The general arterial pressure in man,
as McCurdy 1 has shown, may suddenly rise during extreme physical
effort, from approximately 110 millimeters to 180 millimeters of
mercury.
THE EFFECT OF INCREASING ARTERIAL PRESSURE
What effect the increase of arterial pressure, resulting from
excitement or physical strain, may have on muscular efficiency, has
received only slight consideration. Nice and I found there was need of
careful study of the relations between arterial pressure and muscular
ability, and, in 1913, one of my students, C. M. Gruber, undertook to
make clearer these relations.
The methods of anesthesia and stimulation used by Gruber were
similar to those described in the last chapter. The arterial blood
pressure was registered from the right carotid or the femoral artery by
means of a mercury manometer. A time marker indicating half-minute
intervals was placed at the atmospheric pressure level of the
manometer. And since the blood-pressure style, the writing point of the
muscle lever, and the time signal were all set in a vertical line on
the surface of the recording drum, at any given muscular contraction
the height of blood pressure was simultaneously registered.
To increase general arterial pressure two methods were used:
the spinal cord was stimulated in the cervical region through platinum
electrodes, or the left splanchnic nerves were stimulated after the
left adrenal gland had been excluded from the circulation. This was
done in order to avoid any influence which adrenal secretion might
exert. It is assumed in these experiments that vessels supplying active
muscles would be actively dilated, as Kaufmamx 2 has shown, and would,
therefore, in case of a general increase of blood pressure, deliver a
larger volume of blood to the area they supply. The effects of
increased arterial pressure
arc illustrated in Figs. 13, 14 and 15. In the experiment
represented in Fig. 13, the rise of blood pressure was produced by
stimulation of the cervical cord, and in Figs. 14 and 15 by stimulation
of the left splanchnic nerves after the left adrenal gland had been
tied off.
The original blood pressure in Fig. 13 was 120 millimeters of
mercury. This was increased by 62 millimeters, with a rise of only 8.4
per cent in the height of contraction of the fatigued muscle.
In Fig. 14 the original blood pressure was 100 millimeters of
mercury. By increasing this pressure 32 millimeters there resulted
simultaneous betterment of 9.8 per cent in the height of muscular
contraction. In Fig. 14 B the arterial pressure was raised 26
millimeters and the height of A B C
contraction increased correspondingly 7 per cent. In Fig. 14
C no appreciable betterment can be seen although the blood pressure
rose 18 millimeters.
In Fig. 15 the original blood pressure was low 68 millimeters
of mercury. This was increased in Fig. 15 A by 18 millimeters (the same
as in Fig. 14 C without effect), and there resulted an increase of 20
per cent in the height of contraction.
In Fig. 15 B the pressure was raised 24 millimeters
with a corresponding increase of 90 per cent in the muscular
contraction; and in Fig. 15 C 30 millimeters with a betterment of 125
per cent.
Comparison of Figs. 13, 14 and 15 reveals that the
improvement of contraction of fatigued muscle is much greater when the
blood pressure is raised, even slightly, from a low level, than when it
is raised, perhaps to a very marked degree, from a high level. In one
of the experiments performed by Nice and myself the arterial pressure
was increased by splanchnic stimulation from the low level of 48
millimeters of mercury to 110 millimeters, and the height of the
muscular contractions was increased about sixfold (see Fig. 16).
Results confirming those described above were obtained by
Gruber in a study of the effects of splanchnic stimulation on the
irritability of muscle when fatigued. In a series of eleven
observations the average value of the barely effective stimulus (the
"threshold" stimulus) had to be increased as the condition of fatigue
developed. It was increased for the nerve-muscle by 25 per cent and for
the muscle by 75 per cent. The left
splanchnic nerves, disconnected from the left adrenal gland,
were now stimulated. The arterial pressure,
which had varied between 90 and 100 millimeters of mercury,
was raised at least 40 millimeters. As a result of splanchnic
stimulation there was an average recovery of 42 per cent in the
nerve-muscle and of 46 per cent in the muscle. The increased general
blood pressure was effective, therefore, quite apart from any possible
action of adrenal secretion, in largely restoring to the fatigued
structures their normal irritability.
THE EFFECT OF DECREASING ARTERIAL PRESSURE
Inasmuch as an increase in arterial pressure produces an
increase in the height of contraction of fatigued muscle, it is readily
supposable that a decrease in the pressure would have the opposite
effect. Such is the case only when the blood pressure falls below the
region of 90 to 100 millimeters of mercury. Thus if the arterial
pressure stands at 150 millimeters of mercury, it has to fall
approximately 55 to 65 millimeters before causing a decrease in the
height of contraction. Fig. 17 is the record of an experiment in which
the blood pressure was lowered by lessening the output of
blood from the heart by compressing the thorax. The record shows that
when the pressure was lowered from 120 to 100 millimeters of mercury
(A), there was no appreciable decrease in the height of contraction;
when lowered to 90
millimeters (B), there resulted a decrease of 2.4 per cent;
when to 80 millimeters of mercury (C), a decrease of 7 per cent; and
when to 70 millimeters (D), a decrease of 17.3 per cent. Results
similar to those represented in Fig. 17 were obtained by pulling on a
string looped about the aorta just above its iliac branches, thus
lessening the flow to the hind limbs.
The region of 90 to 100 millimeters of mercury may therefore
be regarded as the critical region at which a falling blood pressure
begins to be accompanied by a concurrent lessening of the efficiency of
muscular contraction, when the muscle is kept in continued activity. It
is at that region that the blood flow is dangerously near to being
inadequate.
AN EXPLANATION OF THE EFFECTS OF VARYING THE ARTERIAL
PRESSURE
How are these effects of increasing and decreasing the
arterial blood pressure most reasonably explained? There is abundant
evidence that fatigue products accumulate in a muscle which is doing
work, and also that these metabolites interfere with efficient
contraction. As Ranke 3 long ago demonstrated, if a muscle, deprived of
circulating blood, is fatigued to a standstill, and then the
circulation is restored, the muscle again responds for a short time to
stimulation, because the waste has been neutralized or swept away by
the fresh blood. When the blood pressure is at its normal
height for warm-blooded animals (about 120 millimeters of mercury, see
Fig. 13), the flow appears to be adequate to wash out the depressive
metabolites, at least in the single muscle used in these experiments,
because a large rise of pressure produces but little change in the
fatigue level. On the other hand, when the pressure is abnormally low,
the flow is inadequate, and the waste products are permitted to
accumulate and clog the action of the muscle. Under such circumstances
a rise of pressure has a very striking beneficial effect.
It is noteworthy that the best results of adrenin on fatigued
muscle reported by previous observers were obtained from studies on
cold-blooded animals. In these animals the circulation is maintained
normally by an arterial pressure about onethird that of warm-blooded
animals. Injection of adrenin in an amount which would not shut off the
blood supply would, by greatly raising the arterial pressure, markedly
increase the circulation of blood in the active muscle. In short, the
conditions in cold-blooded animals are quite like those in the pithed
mammal with an arterial pressure of about 50 millimeters of mercury
(see Fig, 1C). Under
these conditions the improved circulation causes a remarkable
recovery from fatigue. That notable results of adrenin on fatigue are
observed in warm-blooded animals only when they are deeply
anaesthetized or are deprived of the medulla was claimed by Panella. 4
He apparently believed that in normal mammalian conditions adrenin has
little effect because quickly destroyed, whereas in the cold-blooded
animals, and in mammals whose respiratory, circulatory, and thermogenic
states are made similar to the cold-blooded by anaesthesia or pithing,
the contrary is true. In accordance with our observations of the
effects of blood pressure on fatigued muscle, we would explain
Panella's results not as he has done but as due to two factors. First,
the efficiency of the muscle, when blood pressure is low, follows the
ups and downs of pressure much more directly than when the pressure is
high. And second, a given dose of adrenin always raises a low blood
pressure in atonic vessels. The improvement of circulation is capable
of explaining, therefore, the main results obtained in cold-blooded
animals and in pithed mammals.
Oliver and Schafer reported unusually effective contractions
in muscles removed from the body after adrenal extract had been
injected. As shown in Fig. 16, however, the fact that the circulation
had been improved results in continued greater efficiency of the
contracting muscle. Oliver and Schiffer's observation may perhaps be
accounted for on this basis.
THE VALUE OF INCREASED ARTERIAL PRESSURE IN PAIN AND STRONG
EMOTION
As stated in a previous paragraph, there is evidence that the
vessels supplying a muscle dilate when the muscle becomes' active. And
although the normal blood pressure (about 120 millimeters of mercury)
may be able to keep adequately supplied with blood the single muscle
used in our investigation, a higher pressure might be required when
more muscles are involved in activity, for a more widely spread
dilation might then reduce the pressure to the point at which there
would be insufficient circulation in active organs. Furthermore, with
many muscles active, the amount of waste would be greatly augmented,
and the need
for abundant blood supply would thereby to a like degree be
increased. For both reasons a rise of general arterial pressure would
prove advantageous. The high pressure developed in excitement and pain,
therefore, might be specially serviceable in the muscular activities
which are likely to accompany excitement and pain.
In connection with the foregoing considerations, the action
of adrenin on the distribution of blood in the body is highly
interesting. By measuring alterations in the volume of various viscera
and the limbs, Oliver and Schafer 5 proved that the viscera of the
splanchnic area e.g., the spleen, the kidneys, and the intestines
suffer a considerable decrease of volume when adrenin is administered,
whereas the limbs into which the blood is forced from the splanchnic
region actually increase in size. Haskins, Gunning, and Berry 6 showed,
and their work has been confirmed by others, 7 that with nerves intact
adrenin causes active dilatation of the vessels in muscles and
constriction of cutaneous vessels. This action of adrenin indicates
differences in the degree or character of sympathetic innervations. In
other words, at times of pain and excitement sympathetic discharges,
probably aided by the adrenal secretion simultaneously liberated, will
drive the blood out of the vegetative organs of the interior, which
serve the routine needs of the body, into the skeletal muscles which
have to meet by extra action the urgent demands of struggle or escape.
But there are exceptions to the general statement that by
adrenin the viscera are emptied of their blood. It is well known that
adrenin has a vasodilator, not a vasoconstrictor, action on the
arteries of the heart; it is well known also that adrenin affects the
vessels of the brain and the lungs only slightly if at all. From this
evidence we may infer that sympathetic impulses, though causing
constriction of the arteries of the abdominal viscera, have no
effective influence on those of the pulmonary and intracranial areas
and actually
increase the blood supply to the heart. Thus the absolutely
and immediately essential organs those the ancients called the "tripod
of life" the heart, the lungs, the brain (as well as its instruments,
the skeletal muscles) are in times of excitement abundantly supplied
with blood taken from organs of less importance in critical moments.
This shifting of the blood so that there is an assured
adequate supply to structures essential for the preservation of the
individual may reasonably be interpreted as a fact of prime biological
significance. It will be placed in its proper setting when the other
evidence of bodily changes in pain and excitement have been presented.
REFERENCES
1 McCurdy: American Journal of Physiology, 1901, v, p. 98.
2 Kaufmaiin: Archives de Physiologie, 1892, xxiv, p. 283.
3 Ranker Archiv für Anatomic, 18G3, p. 446.
4 Panella: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1907, xlviii, p.
462.
5 Oliver and Schiffer: Journal of Physiology, 1895, xviii, p.
240.
6 Hoskins, Gunning and Berry: American Journal of Physiology,
1916, xli, p. 513.
7 Hartman and Fraser: American Journal of Physiology, 1917,
xliv, p. 353; Gruber: Ibid., 1918, xlv, p. 302 and Pearlman and
Vincent: Endocrinology, 1919, ill, p. 121.
CHAPTER VIII
THE SPECIFIC ROLE OF ADRENIN IN COUNTERACTING THE EFFECTS OF
FATIGUE
As a muscle approaches its fatigue level, its contractions
are decreased in height. Higher contractions will again be elicited if
the stimulus is increased. Although these phenomena are well known, no
adequate analysis of their causes has been advanced. A number of
factors are probably operative in decreasing the height of contraction:
(1) The using up of available energy-producing material; (2) the
accumulation of metabolites in the fatigued muscle; (3) polarization of
the nerve at the point of repeated electrical stimulation; and (4) a
decrease of irritability. It may be that there are interactions between
these factors within the muscle, e. g., the second may cause the
fourth.
VARIATIONS OF THE THRESHOLD STIMULUS AS A MEASURE OF
IRRITABILITY
The last of the factors mentioned - above the effect of
fatigue on the irritability of the nerve-muscle
combination, or on the muscle alone - can no be tested by
determining variations in the least stimulus capable of causing the
slightest contraction, the so-called "threshold stimulus." As the
irritability lessens, the threshold stimulus must necessarily be
higher. The height of the threshold is therefore a measure of
irritability. How does fatigue affect the irritability of nerve-muscle
and muscle ? How is the irritability of fatigued structures affected by
rest? How is it influenced by adrenin or by adrenal secretion? Answers
to
these questions were sought in researches carried on by C. M.
Gruber 1 in 1913.
THE METHOD OF DETERMINING THE THRESHOLD STIMULUS
The neuro-muscular arrangements used in these researches were
in many respects similar to those already described in the account of
experiments by Nice and myself. To avoid the influence of an anesthetic
some of the animals were decerebrated under ether and then used as in
the experiments in which urethane was the anesthetic. The nerve (the
peroneus communis) supplying the tibialis anticus muscle was bared and
severed; and near the cut end shielded platinum electrodes were
applied. These electrodes were used in fatiguing the muscle. Between
these electrodes and the muscle other platinum electrodes could be
quickly applied to determine the threshold stimulus and the tissue
resistance. These second electrodes were removed except when in use,
and when replaced were set always in the same position. Care was taken,
before replacing them, to wipe off moisture on the nerve or on the
platinum points.
For determining the threshold stimulus of the muscle the skin
and other overlying tissues were cut away from the tibialis anticus in
two places about 5 centimeters apart. Through these openings platinum
needle electrodes could be thrust into the muscle whenever readings
were to be taken. Local polarization was avoided by reinserting the
needles into fresh points on the exposed areas whenever new readings
were to be taken.
The tendon of the tibialis anticus was attached, as in the
previous experiments, by a strong thread passing about pulleys to a
lever which when lifted stretched a spring. During the determination of
the threshold the spring was detached from the lever, so that only the
pull of the lever itself (about 15 grams) was exerted on the muscle.
The method of measuring the stimulating value of the electric
current which was used in testing
the threshold was that devised by E. G. Martin* of the
Harvard Laboratory - a method by which the
strength of an induced electric shock is calculable in
definite units. If the tissue resistance enters
* For a full account of Dr. Martin's method of
calculating
the strength of electric stimuli, see Martin: The Measurement of
Induction Shocks, New York, 1912.
into the calculation these are called units. When the
threshold of the nerve-muscle was taken, the apparatus for the
determination was connected with the nerve through the electrodes
nearer the muscle. They were separated from the fatiguing electrodes by
more than 3 centimeters, and arranged so that the kathode was next the
muscle. When the threshold of the muscle was taken directly the
apparatus was connected with the muscle through platinum needle
electrodes thrust into it. The position of the secondary coil of the
inductorium, in every case, was read by moving it away from the primary
coil until the very smallest possible contraction of the muscle was
obtained. Four of these readings were made, one with tissue resistance,
the others with 10,000, 20,000, and 30,000 ohms additional resistance
in the secondary circuit.
Only break shocks were employed the make shocks were
short-circuited. Immediately after the determination of the position of
the secondary coil, and before the electrodes were removed or
disconnected, three readings of the tissue resistance were made. From
these data four values for ft were calculated.
The strength of the primary current for determining the
threshold of the nerve-muscle was usually .01 ampere, but in a few
cases .05 ampere was used. For normal muscle it was .05 ampere and for
denervated muscle 1.0 ampere. The inductorium, which was used
throughout, had a secondary resistance of 1400 ohms. This was added to
the average tissue resistance in making corrections corrections were
made also for core magnetization.
THE LESSENING OF NEURO-MUSCULAR IRRITABILITY BY FATIGUE
The threshold for the peroneus communis nerve in decerebrate
animals varied from 0.319 to 2.96 units, with an average in sixteen
experiments of 1.179.* This average is the same as that found by E. L.
Porter 2 for the radial nerve in the spinal cat. For animals under
urethane anesthesia a higher average was obtained. In these it varied
from .644 to 7.05, or an average in ten experiments of 3.081.
The threshold for the tibialis anticus muscle varied in the
decerebrate animals from 6.75 units to 33.07, or an average in fifteen
experiments of 18.8. Ten experiments were performed under urethane
anesthesia and the threshold varied from 12.53 to 54.9, with an average
of 29.84 β units. From these results it is evident that anesthesia
notably affects the threshold.
E. L. Porter proved, by experiments carried on in the Harvard
Physiological Laboratory, that the threshold of an undisturbed
nerve-muscle remains
* For the detailed data of these and other
quantitative
experiments, the reader should consult the tables in the original
papers.
constant for hours, and his observation was confirmed by
Gruber (see Fig. 19). If, therefore, after fatigue, a change exists in
the threshold, this change is necessarily the result of alterations set
up by the fatigue process in the nerve-muscle or muscle.
After fatigue the threshold of the nerve-muscle, in sixteen
decerebrate animals, increased from an average of 1.179 to 3.34 an
increase of 183 per cent. In ten animals under urethane anesthesia the
threshold after fatigue increased from a normal average of 3.08 to
9.408 an increase of 208 per cent.
An equal increase in the threshold stimulus was obtained from
the normal muscle directly. In decerebrate
animals the normal threshold of 18.8 units was increased by
fatigue to 69.54, or an increase of 274 per cent. With urethane
anesthesia the threshold increased from 29.849 to 66.238, or an
increase of 122 per cent.
Fig. 18, plotted from the data of one of the many
experiments, shows the relative heights of the threshold before and
after fatigue. The correspondence of the two readings of the threshold,
one from the nerve supplying the muscle and the other from the muscle
directly, served as a check on the electrodes. The broken line in the
figure represents the threshold (in units) of the nerve-muscle, and the
continuous line that of the muscle. The threshold values of the
nerve-muscle have been magnified ten times in order to bring the two
records close together. In this experiment the threshold
of the muscle after fatigue (i.e., at 2) is 167 per cent
higher than the normal threshold (at 1), while that of the nerve-muscle
after fatigue is 30.5 per cent higher than its normal.
Evidently a direct relation exists between the duration of
work and the increase of threshold. For instance, the threshold is
higher after a muscle is fatigued for two hours than it is at the end
of the first hour. The relation between the work done and the threshold
is not so clear. In some animals the thresholds were higher after 120
grams had been lifted 120 times a minute for 30 minutes than they were
in others in which 200 grams had been lifted 240 times a minute for the
same period. The muscle in the latter instances did almost four times
as much work, yet the threshold was lower. The difference may be due to
the general condition
of the animal.
A few experiments were performed on animals in which the
nerve supplying the muscle was cut seven to fourteen days previous to
the experiment. The muscle, therefore, had within it no living nerve
fibres. The average normal threshold for the denervated muscle in 6
animals was 61.28 units. As in the normal muscle, the percentage
increase due to fatigue was large.
THE SLOW RESTORATION OF FATIGUED MUSCLE TO NORMAL
IRRITABILITY BY REST
That rest decreases the fatigue threshold of both
nerve-muscle and muscle can be seen in Fig. 18. The time taken for
total recovery, however, is dependent upon the amount of work done, but
this change, like that of fatigue, varies widely with different
individuals. In some animals the threshold returned to normal in 15
minutes; in others, in which the same amount of work was done, it was
still above normal even after 2 hours of rest. This may be due to the
condition of the animals - in some the metabolites are probably
eliminated more rapidly than in others. There were also variations in
the rate of restoration of the normal threshold
when tested on the nerve and when tested on the muscle in the
same animal. In Fig. 18 (at 3) the
nerve-muscle returned to normal in 30 minutes, whereas the
muscle (at 4) after an hour's rest had not returned to normal by a few
ft units. This, however, is not typical of all nerve-muscles and
muscles. The opposite condition that in which the muscle returned to
normal before the nerve-muscle occurred in as many cases as did the
condition just cited. The failure of the two tissues to alter uniformly
in the same direction may be explained as due to variations in the
location of the electrodes when thrust into the muscle at different
times (e. g., whether near nerve filaments or not).
The results from observations made on the nerve are more
likely to be uniform and reliable than are those from the muscle. The
time required for the restoration of the threshold from fatigue to
normal, in denervated
muscles, is approximately the same as that for the normal
muscle.
THE QUICK RESTORATION OP FATIGUED MUSCLE TO NORMAL
IRRITABILITY BY ADRENIN
The foregoing observations showed that fatigue raises the
normal threshold of a muscle, on the average,
between 100 and 200 per cent (it may be increased more than
600 per cent); that this increase is dependent on the time the muscle
works, but also varies with the animal; that rest, 15 minutes to 2
hours, restores the normal irritability; and that this recovery of the
threshold depends upon the time given to rest, the duration of the
work, and also upon the condition of the animal. The problem which was
next attacked by Gruber was that of learning whether the higher
contractions of fatigued muscle after splanchnic stimulation could be
attributed to any influence which adrenal secretion might have in
restoring the normal irritability. To gain insight into the
probabilities he tried first the effects of injecting slowly into the
jugular vein physiological amounts of adrenin.*
The normal threshold of the peroneus communis nerve varied in
the animals used in this series of observations from 0.35 to 5.45
units, with an average in nine experiments of 1.3, a figure close to
the
1.179 found in the earlier series on the effect of fatigue.
For the tibialis anticus muscle, in which the nerve-endings were
intact, the threshold varied
* The form of adrenin used in these and in other
injections
was fresh adrenalin made by Parke, Davis & Co.
from 6.75 to 49.3 units, with an average in the nine
experiments of 22.2. This is slightly higher than that cited for this
same muscle in the earlier series. By fatigue the threshold of the
nerve-muscle was increased from an average of 1.3 to an average of 3.3
units, an increase of 154 per cent. The muscle increased from an
average of 22.2 to an average of 59.6, an increase of 169 per cent.
After an injection of 0.1 to 0.5 cubic centimeters of adrenin
(1:100,000) the fatigue threshold was decreased within five minutes in
the nerve-muscle from an average of 3.3 to 1.8, a recovery of 75 per
cent, and in the muscle from an average of 59.6 to 42.4, a recovery of
46 per cent. To prove that this effect of adrenin is a counteraction
of the effects of fatigue, Gruber determined the threshold for
muscle and nerve-muscle in non-fatigued animals before and
after adrenin injection. He found that in these cases no
lowering of threshold occurred, a result in marked contrast with the
pronounced and prompt lowering induced by this agent in muscles when
fatigued.
Figs. 19 and 20, plotted from the data of two of the
experiments, show the relative heights of the threshold before and
after an injection of adrenin. The close correspondence of the two
readings of the threshold, one from the nerve supplying the muscle, the
other from the muscle directly, served to show that there was no fault
in the electrodes.
The continuous line in the Figures represents the threshold
(in units) of the muscle, the broken line that of the nerve-muscle. The
threshold of the nerve-muscle is magnified 100 times in Fig. 19 and 10
times in Fig. 20. In Fig. 19 (at 2 and 4) the threshold was taken after
an intravenous injection of 0.1 and 0.2 cubic centimeter of adrenin
respectively.
These examples show that adrenin does not affect the
threshold of the normal non-fatigued muscle when tested either on the
muscle directly or on the nerve-muscle. In Fig. 19 (at 3) the
observation taken after two hours of rest illustrates the constancy of
the threshold under these circumstances.
In Fig. 19 the normal threshold was increased by fatigue (at
5) the muscle had been pulling 120 times a minute for one hour on a
spring having an initial tension of 120 grams from 30.0 to 51.6 units,
an increase of 72 per cent; and in the nerve-muscle from 0.62 to 0.89
units, an increase of 46 per cent. The threshold (at 6)
was taken fire minutes after injecting 0.1 cubic centimeter
of adrenin (1:100,000). The threshold of the muscle was lowered from
51.6 to 38.0 units, a recovery of 62 per cent; that of the nerve-muscle
from 0.89 to 0.79 units, a recovery of 37 per cent. After another
injection of 0.5 cubic centimeter of adrenin the thresholds (at 7)
weretaken; that of the nerve-muscle dropped to normal 0.59 units a
recovery of 100 per cent, and that
of the muscle remained unaltered 26 per cent above its normal
threshold.
In Fig. 20 the threshold (at 5) was taken five minutes after
an injection of 0.1 cubic centimeter of adrenin. The drop here was as
large as that shown in Fig. 19. The threshold taken from the
muscle directly was lowered from 30.6 to 18 units, a recovery
of 61 per cent; the nerve-muscle from 1.08 to 0.87 units, a recovery of
51 per cent. That this sudden decrease cannot be due to rest is shown
in the same Figure (at 3 and 4). These readings were made after 60 and
90 minutes' rest respectively. The sharp decline in the record (at 5)
indicates distinctly the remarkable restorative influence of adrenin in
promptly lowering the high, fatigue threshold of neuro-muscular
irritability.
THE EVIDENCE THAT THE KESTORATIVE ACTION OF ADRENIN IS
SPECIFIC
As stated in describing the effects of arterial blood
pressure, an increase of pressure is capable of causing a decided
lowering of the neuro-muscular threshold after fatigue. Is it not
possible that adrenin produces its beneficial effects by bettering the
circulation ?
Nice and I had argued that the higher contractions of
fatigued muscle, that follow stimulation or injection of adrenin, could
not be wholly due to improved blood flow through the muscle, for when
by traction on the aorta or compression of the thorax arterial pressure
in the hind legs was prevented from rising, splanchnic stimulation
still caused a distinct improvement, the initial appearance of which
coincided with the point in the bloodpressure curve at which evidence
of adrenal secretion appeared. And, furthermore, the improvement was
seen also when adrenin was given intravenously in such weak solution
(1:100,000) as to
produce a fall instead of a rise of arterial
pressure. Lyman and I had shown that this fall of pressure was due to a
dilator effect of adrenin. Since the blood vessels of the fatigued
muscle were dilated by severance of their nerves when the nerve trunk
was cut, and, besides, as previously stated (see p. 86), were being
stimulated through their nerves at a rate favorable to relaxation, it
seemed hardly prob-
able that adrenin could produce its beneficial effect by
further dilation of the vessels and by consequent
flushing of the muscle with an extra supply of blood, 3 The
lowering of blood pressure had been proved to have no other effect than
to impair the action of the muscle (see p. 103) . Although the chances
were thus against an interpretation of the beneficial influence of
adrenin through action on the circulation, it was thought desirable to
test the possibility by comparing its effect with that of another
vasodilator amyl nitrite.
Figs. 21 and 22 are curves obtained from the left tibialis
anticus muscle. The rate of stimulation was 240 times a minute.
The muscle in Fig. 21 contracted against a spring having an
initial tension of 120 grams, and that in Fig. 22 against an initial
tension of 100 grams. In Fig. 21, at the point indicated on the base
line, 0.4 cubic centimeter of adrenin (1:100,000) was injected into the
left external jugular vein. There resulted a fall of 25 millimeters of
mercury in the arterial pressure and a concurrent betterment of 15 per
cent in the height of contraction, requiring two minutes and fifteen
seconds of fatigue (about 540 contractions) before it returned to the
former level. In Fig. 22, at the point indicated by the arrow, a
solution of amyl nitrite was injected into the right external jugular
vein. There resulted a fall of 70 millimeters of mercury in arterial
pressure and a betterment of 4.1 per cent in the height of muscular
contraction, requiring fifteen seconds
of fatigue (about 60 contractions) to decrease the height of
contraction to its former level. In
neither case did the blood pressure fall below the critical
region (see p. 104).* Although the fall in arterial pressure caused by
dilation of the vessels due to amyl nitrite was almost three times as
great as that produced by the adrenin, yet the resultant betterment was
only about one-fourth the percentage height and lasted but one-ninth
the time. In all cases in which these solutions caused an equal fall in
arterial pressure, adrenin caused higher contractions, whereas amyl
nitrite caused no appreciable change.
THE POINT OF ACTION OF ADRENIN IN MUSCLE
From the evidence presented in the foregoing pages it is
clear that adrenin somehow is able to bring about a rapid recovery of
normal irritability of muscle after the irritability has been much
lessened by fatigue, and that the higher contractions of a fatigued
muscle after an injection of adrenin are due, certainly in part, to
some specific action of this substance and not wholly to its influence
on the circulation. Some of the earlier investigators
* In some cases after injection of amyl nitrite the
normal
blood pressure, which was high, dropped sharply to a point below the
critical region. There resulted a primary increase in muscular
contraction due to the betterment in circulation caused by the dilation
of the vessels before the critical region was reached. During the time
that the pressure was below the critical region the muscle contraction
fell. As the blood pressure again rose to normal the muscle contraction
increased coincidently.
of adrenal function, notably Albanese, 4 and also Abelous and
Langlois, 5 inferred from experiments on the removal of the glands that
the role they played in the bodily economy was that of neutralizing,
destroying or transforming toxic substances produced in the organism as
a result of muscular or nervous work. It seemed possible that the
metabolites might have a checking or blocking influence at the junction
of the nerve fibres with the muscle fibres, and might thus, like
curare, lessen the efficiency of the nerve impulses. Radwanska's
observation 6 that the beneficial action of adrenin is far greater when
the muscle is stimulated through its nerve than when stimulated
directly, and Panella's discovery 7 that adrenin antagonizes the effect
of curare, were favorable to the view that adrenin improves the
contraction of fatigued muscle by lessening or removing a block
established by accumulated metabolites.
The high threshold of fatigued denervated muscle, however,
Gruber found was quite as promptly lowered by adrenin as was that of
normal muscles stimulated through their nerves. Fig. 23 shows that the
height of contraction, also, of the fatigued muscle is increased when
adrenin is administered. In this experiment the left tibialis anticus
muscle was stimulated directly by thrusting platinum needle electrodes
into it. The peroneus communis nerve supplying the muscle had been cut
and two centimeters of it removed nine days previous to the experiment.
The rate of stimulation was 120 times per minute and the initial
tension of the
spring about 120 grams. At the point indicated
by the arrow an injection of 0.1 cubic centimeter of adrenin
(1:100,000) was made into a jugular vein. A fall in arterial pressure
from 110 to 86 millimeters of mercury and a simultaneous betterment of
20 per cent in the height of contraction were obtained. It required
four minutes of fatigue (about 480 contractions) to restore the muscle
curve to its former level. Results similar to this were obtained from
animals in which the nerve had been cut 7, 9, 12, 14, and 21 days. In
all instances the nerve was inexcitable to strong faradic stimulation.
In Radwanska's experiments, mentioned above, the muscle was
stimulated directly when the nerve endings were intact. It seems
reasonable to suppose, therefore, that in all cases he was stimulating
nerve tissue. Since a muscle is more irritable when stimulated through
its nerve than when stimulated directly (nerve and muscle), a slight
change in the irritability of the muscle by adrenin would naturally
result in a greater contraction when the nerve was stimulated.
Panella's results also are not inconsistent with the interpretation
that the effect of adrenin is on the muscle substance rather
than on the nerve endings. A method which has long been used to
separate muscle from nerve is that of blocking the nervous impulses by
the drug curare. Gruber found that when curare is injected the
threshold of the normal muscle is increased, as was to be expected from
the removal of the highly efficient nervous stimulations. And also, as
was to be expected on that basis, curare did not increase the threshold
in a muscle in which the nerve endings had degenerated. Adrenin
antagonizes curare with great promptness, decreasing the heightened
threshold of a curarized muscle, in five minutes or less, in some cases
to normal. From this observation it might be supposed that curare and
fatigue had the same effect, and that adrenin had the single action of
opposing that effect. But
fatigue raises the threshold of a curarized muscle, and
adrenin then antagonizes this fatigue. Langley 8 has argued that curare
acts upon a hypothetical "receptive substance" in muscle. If so,
probably curare acts upon a substance, or at a point, different from
that upon which fatigue acts; for, as the foregoing evidence shows,
fatigue increases the threshold of a muscle whether deprived of its
nerve supply by nerve section and degeneration or by curare, whereas
curare affects only the threshold of a muscle in which the nerve
endings
are normal.9And since adrenin can oppose the effects of both
curare and fatigue, it may be said to have two actions, or to act on
two different substances or at two different points in the muscle.
Gruber 10 has recently shown that adrenin perfused through
dying muscle, and through muscle rendered less efficient by the
injection of fatigue products (lactic acid, and acid sodium and
potassium phosphate), has a remarkable capacity to restore the
contractile process after it has practically disappeared, or to augment
it greatly after it has been much reduced.
The evidence adduced in the last chapter indicated that the
greater "head" of arterial pressure produced by the more rapid heart
beat and the stronger contraction of many arterioles in times of great
excitement would be highly serviceable to the organism in any extensive
muscular activity which the excitement might involve. By assuring an
abundant flow of blood through the enlarged vessels of the working
muscle, the waste products resulting from the wear and tear in
contraction would be promptly swept away and thus would be prevented
from impairing the muscular efficiency. The adrenin discharge at such
times would, as was pointed out, probably reinforce the effects of
sympathetic impulses. The evidence presented in this chapter shows that
adrenin has also another action, a very remarkable action, that of
restoring to a muscle its original ability to respond to stimulation,
after that has been largely lost by continued activity through a long
period. What rest will do only after an hour or more, adrenin will do
in five minutes or less. The bearing of this striking phenomenon on the
functions of the organism in times of great need for muscular activity
will be considered in a later discussion.
REFERENCES
1 Gruber: American Journal of Physiology, 1913, xxxii, p.
437.
2 E. L. Porter: ' American Journal of Physiology, 1912, xxxi,
p. 149.
3 Cannon and Nice: American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxii, p. 55.
4 Albanese: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1892, xvii, p.
239.
5 Abelous and Langlois: Archives de Physiologie, 1892, xxiv,
pp. 269-278, 465-476.
6 Radwanska: Anzeiger der Akademie, Krakau, 1910, pp.
728-736. Reviewed in the Centralblatt für Biochemie und Biophysik,
1911, xi, p. 467.
7 Panella: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1907, xlvii, p.
30.
8 Langley: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 1906,
Ixxviii, B, p. 181. Journal of Physiology, 1905-6, xxxiii, pp. 374-413.
9 See Gruber: American Journal of Physiology, 1914, xxxiv, p.
89.
10 Gruber: Ibid., 1918, xlvi, p. 472; 1918, xlvii, p. 178,
185.
CHAPTER IX
THE HASTENING OF COAGULATION OF BLOOD BY ADRENIN
The primary value of blood to the body must have been one of
the earliest observations of reasoning beings. When we consider the
variety of fundamental services which this circulating fluid performs
the conveyance of food and oxygen to all the tissues, the removal of
waste, the delivery of the internal secretions, the protection of the
body against toxins and bacterial invasion, and the distribution of
heat from active to inactive regions - the view of the ancient Hebrews
that the "life of the flesh is in the blood" is well justified. It is
naturally of the utmost importance that this precious fluid shall be
safeguarded against loss. And its property of turning to a jelly soon
after escaping from its natural channels assures a closure of the
opening through which the escape occurred, and thus protection of the
body from further bleeding. The slight evidence that adrenin hastens
the clotting process has already been hinted at. When we found that
adrenin is set free in pain and intense emotion, it seemed possible
that there might exist in the body an arrangement for making doubly
sure the assurance against loss of blood, a process that might nicely
play its role precisely when the greatest need for it would be likely
to arise.
It was in 1903, while tracing in dogs the rise and fall of
sugar in the blood after administering adrenin, that Vosburgh and
Richards 1 first noted that simultaneously with the increase of blood
sugar there occurred more rapid coagulation. In some cases the
diminution was as much as fourfifths the coagulation time of the
control. Since this result was obtained by painting "adrenalin" on the
pancreas, as well as by injecting it into the abdominal cavity, they
concluded that "the phenomenon appears to be due to the application of
adrenalin to the pancreas." Six years later, during a study of the
effect of adrenalin on internal hemorrhage, Wiggers 2 examined
incidentally the evidence presented by Vosburgh and Richards, and after
many tests on five dogs found "never the slightest indication that
adrenalin, either when injected or added to the blood, appreciably
hastened the coagulation process." In 1911 von den Velden 3 reported
that adrenin (about 0.007 milligram
per kilo of body weight) decreased the coagulation time in
man about one-half an effect appearing 11 minutes after administration
by mouth, and 85 minutes after subcutaneous injection. He affirmed
also, but without describing the conditions or giving figures, that
adrenin decreases coagulation time in vitro. He. did not attribute the
coagulative effect of adrenin in patients to this direct action on the
blood, however, but to
vasoconstriction disturbing the normal circulation and
thereby the normal equilibrium between blood
and tissue. In consequence, the tissue juices with their
coagulative properties enter the blood, so he assumed. In support of
this theory he offered his observation that coagulation time is
decreased after the nasal mucosa has been rendered anemic by adrenin
pledgets. Von den Velden's claim 3 for adrenin given by mouth was
subjected to a single test on man by Dale and Laidlaw, 4 but their
result was completely negative.
The importance of Vosburgh and Richards' observation, the
thoroughly discordant testimony of later investigators, as well as the
meager and incidental nature of all the evidence that has been adduced
either for or against the acceleration of clotting by adrenin, made
desirable a further study of this matter. Especially was this further
study desirable because of the discharge of adrenin into the blood in
pain and emotional excitement. Accordingly, in 1914, H. Gray and I 5
undertook an investigation of the question. In doing so we employed
cats as subjects. Usually they were quickly decerebrated under ether,
and then continuance of
the drug became unnecessary. Body temperature was maintained
by means of an electric heating pad. Respiration proceeded normally
except in a few instances (in which, presumably, there was hemorrhage
into the medulla), when artificial respiration had to be given.
THE GRAPHIC METHOD OP MEASURING THE COAGULATION TIME
In order to avoid, so far as possible, the personal element
in determining when the blood was clotted,
the blood was made to record its own clotting. The instrument
by means of which this was done was
the graphic coagulometer devised by W. L. Mendenhall and
myself, 6 and illustrated diagrammatically in Fig. 24. It consists
essentially of a light aluminum lever with the long arm nearly
counterpoised by a weight W. The long arm is prevented from falling by
a support S, and is prevented from rising by.a horizontal right-angled
rod reaching over the lever at Rl and fixed into the block B which
turns on the axis A. Into the same
block is fixed the vertical rod R2 . When this rod is moved
from the post P1 , against which it is held
by the weight of the horizontal rod R1 towards the other post
P2 , the check on the long arm of the lever is lifted, and if the short
arm is heavier, the long arm will then rise.
The cannula C, into which the blood is received, is two
centimeters in total length and slightly more than two millimeters in
internal diameter. It is attached by a short piece of rubber tubing to
the tapered glass tube T, five centimeters long and five millimeters in
internal diameter. The upper end of this tube is surrounded by another
piece of rubber which supports the tube when it is slid into the
U-shaped support U, fixed directly below the end of the short arm of
the lever.
By drawing the cannulas from a single piece of glass tubing
and by making the distance from shoulder to upper end about twelve
millimeters, receptacles of fairly uniform capacity are assured. All
the dimensions, the reach of the rubber connection over the top of the
cannula (2-3 millimeters), the distance of the upper rubber ring from
the lower end of the glass chamber (4 centimeters), etc., were as
nearly standard as possible.
A copper wire D, eight centimeters long and 0.6 millimeters
in diameter, bent above into a hook and below into a small ring
slightly less than two millimeters in diameter, is hung in a depression
at the end of the short arm of the lever. The small ring then rests in
the upper part of the cannula (see Fig. 24). The weight of the copper
wire makes the short arm of the lever heavier than the long arm by 30
milligrams, when the delicate writing point is moving over a lightly
smoked drum. Half a dozen of these standard wires are needed.
For accurate determination of the coagulation time Addis 7
has defined the following conditions as essential:
1. The blood must always be obtained under the same
conditions.
2. Estimates must all be made at the same temperature.
3. The blood must always come in contact with the same amount
and kind of foreign material.
4. The end point must be clear and definite and must always
indicate the same degree of coagulation.
The precautions taken to fulfill these conditions were as
follows:
1. Drawing the blood. - The blood was taken from
the femoral artery. The artery (usually the right) was laid bare in the
groin and freed from surrounding tissue. A narrow artery clip, with
each limb enclosed in soft rubber tubing (to prevent injury of the
tissues), and with its spring exerting gentle pressure, was placed on
the artery immediately below the deep femoral branch, thus allowing no
blood to stagnate above the clip. Between the clip and a ligature
applied about 1.5 centimeters below, an opening was made. The blood was
carefully milked out of the vessels between a blunt dissector moved
beneath, and a small forceps, twisted into a pinch of absorbent cotton,
moved above.
The cannula, cleaned in water, alcohol, and ether, was set in
the rubber connection of the glass tube; the point of the cannula was
then lubricated with vaseline and slipped into the artery. The pressure
of the clip on the artery was next very slightly released and blood was
allowed to flow into the cannula up to the lower border of the rubber
connection. Only a good-sized drop of blood was needed. Sometimes the
blood ran one or two millimeters above or below, but without
appreciably changing the result. Since the clip was situated on the
femoral immediately below a branch in which the circulation persisted, the
blood received in the
cannula was always fresh from the moving stream. As
soon as the clip gripped the artery again, the cannula was slipped out.
A helper then promptly milked the vessel in the manner described above,
and covered it with a pad of absorbent cotton smeared with vaseline to
prevent drying. Thereby blood was not permitted to stagnate; and when a
new sample was to be taken, the vessel was clean and ready for use.
The tip of the cannula was at once plugged by plunging it
into a flat mound of plasticine about three millimeters high. It was
drawn off sidewise lest the plasticine plug be pulled out again. One of
the copper wires D was now slid into the tube and cannula, the tube
slipped into the U-support, and the wire lifted and hung on the lever.
This procedure, from the moment blood began to flow until the wire was
hung, consumed usually about twenty seconds.
2. Uniform temperature. Under the U-support was
placed a large water bath, in which the cannula and the tapering part
of the tube were submerged. A thermometer was fixed to the U-support so
that the bulb came near the cannula in the bath. The water was kept
within a degree of 25 C. This temperature was chosen for several
reasons: (a) The cannula has room temperature and rapidly cools the
small volume of blood that enters it. To heat blood and cannula to body
temperature would take time. A bath near room temperature,
therefore, seems preferable to one near body temperature, (b)
The test of clotting was conveniently made at intervals of a
half-minute, and if the clotting process were hastened by higher
temperatures, this interval would become relatively less exact, (c) A
temperature of 25 C. rather than lower was selected because, as Dale
and Laidlaw 8 have shown, the coagulation time is much slower for
a given change in temperature below 25 than for the same change above.
And with slowing of the process the end point, when the determination
depends on supporting a weight, is less likely to be sharp, (d) The
researches undertaken with
use of this coagulometer were concerned with factors
hastening the process. For that reason and for reason (b), a long
rather than a short coagulation time for normal conditions was
desirable.
3. Uniformity in the amount and kind of contact with
foreign surface. The capacity of the cannulas was fairly uniform,
as stated above; the amount received in them was fairly constant; and
the wire hanging in the blood presented approximately the same surface
in different observations.
A further condition for insuring consistent treatment of the
blood in different cases was that of making the tests for coagulation
always at the same intervals. Below the writing point of the lever was
set an electromagnetic signal E, which recorded half-minutes. At the
moment a record was made by the signal (see first signal mark, Fig. 25)
the clip on the artery was opened, the blood taken, and the process
thus begun. In about 20 seconds the cannula was suspended in the water
bath and the wire was hanging on the lever. At the next
record by the signal and at every subsequent record the vertical rod R2
was pushed with the index finger from post Pl to post P2 and allowed to
move back. This motion was uniform and lasted
about one second. The check Rl on the long arm of the lever
was thus raised, and as the wire sank in the blood the writing point
rose, recording that coagulation had not taken place (see Fig. 25).
4. Definite end point. As soon as the blood
clotted, the weight of 30 milligrams was supported, and the failure of
the lever to rise to the former height in the regular time allowed,
recorded that the change had occurred.
Very rarely the swing of the lever would be checked for a
moment and would then begin to move rapidly, indicating that a strand
of fibrin had formed but not sufficiently strong to support the weight,
and that when the strand broke, the weight quickly sank in the blood.
If this occurred, the next record almost always was the short line,
which signified that the weight was well supported.
A very slight strand of fibrin was able to prevent the weight
from dropping, though at different times the amount of support
differed, as shown by the varying length of the final lines (compare
first and last series, Fig. 25). These variations are probably a rough
indication of the degree of coagulation. In our experiments, however,
the length of the final line was disregarded, and merely the fact that
the lever failed to swing trough its usual distance was taken as
evidence of a clot, and the consequent short record was taken as the
end point.
As soon as this end point was registered, the tube, wire and
cannula were lifted out of the bath; the cannula was then separated
from the tube and pulled away from the wire. The clot was thus
disclosed, confirming the graphic record.
The method, at least when used at half-minute intervals, did
not reveal in all instances the same degree of clotting. Usually, when
the process was very rapid, the revealed clot was a thick jelly;
whereas, when the process was slow, a strand of fibrin or at most a
small amount of jelly was found. This difference in the degree of
coagulation introduced, of course, an element of inexactness. In our
experiments, however, this inexactness was unfavorable to the result we
were seeking for, i. e., the acceleration of the process because the
jelly is a later stage than the fibrin strand; and since we
nevertheless obtained good evidence of acceleration, we did not in
these experiments attempt to determine more accurately differences in
the stage
of the clotting process,
5. Cleaning of apparatus. After the wire was
removed from the tube, the clot attached to its ring-tip was carefully
brushed away under cool running water. Under the running water, also, a
trimmed feather was introduced into the cannula and the tube to push
out the plasticine and to wash out the blood. Wire, cannula and tube
were then dropped into a beaker receiving running hot water (about 80
C.) and there allowed to remain for about five minutes. On removal from
this the parts were shaken free from water, passed through 95 per cent
alcohol and again shaken free, passed through ether and let dry.
By having a half-dozen cannulas and wires of standard size,
it was possible to save trouble by cleaning a number at one time.
Not infrequently the first few samples of blood taken from an
animal showed rapid or somewhat irregular rates of clotting. Some
causes for these initial variations will be presented in following
pages. The fairly uniform rate of clotting in any individual after the
initial stage, varied in twenty-one different animals from an average
of 3 to an average of 10.6 minutes, with a combined average of 5.9
minutes. The conditions for these variations among the individuals have
not been wholly determined.
THE EFFECTS OF SUBCUTANEOUS INJECTIONS OF ADRENIN
The first observations were of this class.
Oct. 27. A cat weighing about 3 kilos was given 3 cubic
centimeters of adrenin 1:1,000, i.e., 1 milligram
per kilo, under the skin. The animal, in this instance, was
kept in uniform ether anesthesia. Following is a record showing when
blood was taken, and the coagulation time in each instance:
In this case the coagulation time remained at its usual level
for about 20 minutes after the subcutaneous
injection.* Thereafter for about an hour the coagulation time
averaged 45 per cent of its previous duration. And widely separated
tests made during the following hour indicated that approximately the
initial rate of clotting had been regained. The rather long period
(nearly 30 minutes), in the case just cited, between the injection and
the
* This period is longer than is expected after the
subcutaneous injection of any drug. As will be shown later, strong
doses of adrenin, if injected rapidly, may not at first shorten the
clotting process. Probably in some instances of subcutaneous injection
of these strong doses, the drug enters the circulation more rapidly
than in others and in consequence coagulation is not at first
accelerated.
first appearance of rapid clotting was not the rule. As the
following figures show, the coagulation time
may become shortened quite promptly after subcutaneous
injection.
In this case nine minutes after the injection the change in
the rate of clotting had begun, and it continued
more rapid for the subsequent half-hour. We did not attempt
to find the- minimal subcutaneous dose which would shorten clotting. A
dose of 0.01 milligram per kilo, however, has proved effective, as
shown by the following figures:
As will be shown later, the dose in this instance was ten
times the minimal effective intravenous dose. On the basis of these
figures, less than a milligram of adrenin given subcutaneously would be
necessary to shorten clotting to a marked degree in a man of average
weight (70 kilograms).
Not many observations were made by us on the effects of
adrenin administered subcutaneously. The amount reaching the vascular
system and the rate of its entrance into the blood could be so much
more accurately controlled by intravenous than by subcutaneous
introduction that most of our attention was devoted to the former
method.
THE EFFECTS OF INTRAVENOUS INJECTIONS
In this procedure a glass cannula was fastened in one of the
external jugular veins and filled with the same solution as that to be
injected. A short rubber tube was attached and tightly clamped close to
the glass. Later, for the injection, the syringe needle was inserted
through the rubber and into the fluid in the cannula, the clip on the
vein was removed, and the injection made.
The solutions employed intravenously were adrenin 1:10,000,
1:50,000, and 1:100,000, in distilled water.
The smallest amount which produced any change in clotting
time was 0.1 cubic centimeter of a dilution of 1:100,000 in
a cat weighing two kilos, a dose of 0.0005 milligram per kilo. Four
tests previous to the injection averaged 5 minutes, and none was
shorter than 4 minutes. Immediately after the injection the time was 2
minutes, but at the next test the effect had disappeared. Doubling the
dose in the same cat i. e., giving 0.2 cubic centimeter (0.001
milligram per kilo) shortened the coagulation time for about 40
minutes:
From 10.47, immediately after the second injection, till
11.20 the average time for clotting was 2.5 minutes, whereas both
before and after this period the time was 4 minutes or longer. At 11.00
o'clock and 11.05, when the end point was reached in 1.5 minutes ( a
reduction of 63 per cent), a thick jelly was found on examining the
cannula. The changes in clotting time in this case are represented
graphically in Fig. 26.
In another case a dose of 0.0005 milligram per kilo failed to
produce any change, but 0.001 milligram
per kilo (0.28 cubic centimeter of adrenin, 1:100,000, given
a cat weighing 2.8 kilos) brought a
sharp decline in the record, as follows:
In these instances the animals were decerebrated. For
decerebrate cats, the least amount of adrenin, intravenously, needed to
produce shortening of coagulation time is approximately 0.001 milligram
per kilo.
In the above cases rapid clotting was manifest directly after
minute doses. Larger doses, however
may produce primarily not faster clotting but slower, and
that may be followed in turn by a much shorter coagulation time. The
figures below present such an instance:
This unexpected primary increase of coagulation time, lasting
at least six minutes, is in striking contrast to the later remarkable
shortening of the process from 3 to an average of 1.7 minutes for more
than 20 minutes (see Fig. 27, A).
If a strong solution, i. e., 1:10,000, is injected rapidly,
the process may be prolonged as above, but not followed as above by
shortening, thus:
There was in this case no decrease in coagulation time at any
test for a half-hour after the injection, but instead a lengthening
(see Fig. 27, B). Howell 9 has reported the interesting observation
that repeated massive doses of adrenin given to dogs may so greatly
retard coagulation that the animals may be said to be hemophilic. These
two instances show that on coagulation large doses have the contrary
effect to small, just as Hoskins 10 showed was true for intestinal and
Lyman and I 11 showed was true for arterial smooth muscle.
In a few experiments the brain and the cord to midthorax were
destroyed through the orbit. Artificial respiration then maintained the
animal in uniform condition. Under these circumstances, adrenin
intravenously had more lasting effects than when given to the usual
decerebrate animals with intact cord. Fig. 28 illustrates such a case.
For thirty minutes before injection the clotting time averaged 5.4
minutes. Then, about ten minutes after one cubic centimeter of adrenin,
1:50,000, had
been slowly injected, clotting began to quicken; during the
next twenty minutes the average was 3.4 minutes, and during the
following forty-five minutes the average was 1.9 minutes only 35 per
cent as long as it had been before the injection.
In another case in which the brain and upper cord were
similarly destroyed, the clotting time, which for a half-hour had
averaged 3.9 minutes, was reduced by one cubic centimeter of adrenin,
1:100,000, to an average for the next hour and forty minutes of 2.3
minutes, with 1.5 and 3 minutes as extremes. During the first forty
minutes of this period of one hour and forty minutes of rapid clotting
all of eight tests except two showed a coagulation time of 2 minutes or
less. The explanation of this persistent rapid clotting in animals with
spinal cord pithed is not yet clear.
As indicated in Figs. 26, 27 and 28, the records of
coagulation show oscillations. Some of these ups and downs are, of
course, within the limits of
error of the method, but in our experience they have occurred
so characteristically after injection of adrenin, and so often have
appeared in a rough rhythm, that they have given the impression of
being real accompaniments of faster clotting. It may be that two
factors are operating, one tending to hasten, the other to retard the
process, and that the equilibrium disturbed by adrenin is recovered
only after interaction to and fro between the two factors.
The oscillations in coagulation time after the injections
suggest that clotting might vary with changes in blood pressure, for
that also commonly oscillates after a dose of adrenin (see, e.g., Fig.
23). Simultaneous recording of blood pressure and determining of
coagulation time have revealed that each may vary without corresponding
variation in the other. Within ordinary limits, therefore, changes of
blood pressure do not change the rate of clotting.
THE HASTENING OF COAGULATION BY ADRENIN NOT A DIRECT EFFECT
ON THE BLOOD
As previously stated, von den Velden has contended that
shortening of coagulation time by adrenin is due to exudation of tissue
juices resulting from vasoconstriction. The amount of adrenin which
produces markedly faster clotting in the cat, is approximately 0.001
milligram per kilo. As Lyman and I 12 showed, however, this amount when
injected slowly, as in the present experiments, results in brief
vasodilation rather than
vasoconstriction. Von den Velden's explanation can therefore
not be applied to these experiments.
He has claimed, furthermore, that adrenin added to blood in
vitro makes it clot more rapidly, but, as already noted, he gives no
account of the conditions of his experiments and no figures. It is
impossible, therefore, to criticise them. His claim, however, is
contrary to Wiggers's 13 earlier observations that blood with added
adrenin coagulated no more quickly than blood with an equal amount of
added physiological salt solution. Also contrary to this claim are the
following two experiments: (1) Ligatures were tied around the aorta and
inferior vena cava immediately above the diaphragm, and thus the
circulation was confined almost completely to the anterior part of the
animal. Indeed, since the posterior part ceases to function in the
absence of blood supply, the preparation may be called an "anterior
animal." When such a preparation was made and 0.5 cubic centimeter of
adrenin, 1:100,000 (half the usual dose, because, roughly, half an
animal), was injected slowly into one of the jugulars, coagulation was
not shortened. "Whereas for a half-hour before the injection the
clotting time averaged 4.6 minutes, for an hour thereafter the average
was 5.3 minutes a prolongation which may have been due, not to any
influence of adrenin, but to failure of the blood to circulate through
the intestines and liver.14 In an other experiment after the
gastro-intestinal canal and liver had been removed from the animal, the
average time for coagulation during twenty-five minutes before
injecting adrenin (0.23 cubic centimeter, 1:100,000, in an animal
weighing originally 2.3 kilos) was 5.5 minutes, and during forty
minutes after the injection it was 6.8 minutes, with no case shorter
than 6 minutes. In the absence of circulation through the abdominal
viscera, therefore, adrenin fails to shorten the clotting time. (2) The
cannulas were filled with adrenin, 1:1,000, and emptied just before
being introduced into the
artery. The small amount of adrenin left on the walls was
thus automatically mixed with the drawn blood. Alternate observations
with these cannulas wet by adrenin and with the usual dry cannulas
showed no noteworthy distinction.
The results of these experiments have made it impossible for
us to concede either of von den Velden's claims, i. e., that clotting
occurs faster because adrenin is added to the blood, or because adrenin
by producing vasoconstriction causes tissues to exude coagulant juices.
Vosburgh and Richards found that coagulation became more
rapid as the blood sugar increased. Conceivably faster clotting might
result from this higher percentage of blood sugar. Against this
assumption, however, is the fact that clotting is greatly accelerated
by 0.001 milligram adrenin per kilo of body weight, much less than the
dose necessary to increase the sugar content of the blood.15 And
furthermore, when dextrose (3 cubic centimeters of a 10 per cent
solution) is added to the blood of an anterior animal, making the blood
sugar roughly 0.3 per cent, the coagulation time is not markedly
reduced. Adrenin appears to act,
therefore, in some other way than by increasing blood sugar.
Since adrenin makes the blood clot much faster than normally
in the intact animal, and fails to have this effect when the
circulation is confined to the anterior animal, the inference is
justified that in the small doses here employed adrenin produces its
remarkable effects, not directly on the blood itself, not through
change in the extensive neuromuscular, bony, or surface tissues of the
body, but through some organ in the abdomen.
That exclusion of the liver from the bodily economy, by
ligature of its vessels or by phosphorus poisoning, will result in
great lengthening of the coagulation time has been clearly shown. The
liver, therefore, seems to furnish continuously to the blood a factor
in the clotting process which is being continuously destroyed in the
body. It is not unlikely that adrenin makes the blood clot more rapidly
by stimulating the liver to discharge this factor in greater abundance.
But proof for this suggestion has not yet been established.
REFERENCES
1 Vosburgh and Richards: American Journal of Physiology,
1903, ix, p. 39.
2 Wiggers: Archives of Internal Medicine, 1909, iii, p. 152.
3 Von den Velden: Münchener medizinische Wochenschrift,
1911,
Iviii, p. 1ST.
4 Dale and Laidlaw: Journal of Pathology and Bacteriology,
1912, xvi, p. 362.
5 Cannon and Gray: American Journal of Physiology, 1914,
xxxiv, p. 321.
6 Cannon and Mendenhall: American Journal of Physiology,
1914, xxxiv, p. 225.
7 Addis: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology, 1908,
i, p. 314.
8 Dale and Laidlaw: Loc. cit., p. 359.
9 Howell: American Journal of Physiology, 1914, xxxiii, p.
xiv.
10 Hoskins: American Journal of Physiology, 1912, xxix, p.
365.
11 Cannon and Lyman: American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxi, p. 376.
12 Cannon and Lyman: Loc. cit., p. 381.
13 Wiggers: Loc. cit., p. 152.
14 See Pawlow: Archiv für Physiologie, 1887, p. 458.
Bohr:
Centralblatt für Physiologie, 1888, ii, p. 263. Meek: American
Journal
of Physiology, 1912, xxx, p. 173. Gray and Lunt: Hid., 1914, xxxiv, p.
332.
15 Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1914, xxxiii, p.
396.
CHAPTER X
THE HASTENING OF THE COAGULATION OF BLOOD IN PAIN AND GREAT
EMOTION
In the foregoing chapter evidence was presented that the
intravenous injection of minute amounts of adrenin hastens the clotting
of blood. The amounts used did not vary much above or below the amounts
discharged by the adrenal glands after brief stimulation of the
splanchnic nerves, as found by H. Osgood in the Harvard Laboratory, and
may therefore be regarded as physiological. Since injected adrenin is
capable of shortening the coagulation time, may not the increased
secretion of the adrenals likewise have that effect? The answer
to this question was the object of an investigation by W. L.
Mendenhall and myself. 1
The blood was taken and its coagulation was recorded
graphically in the manner already described. In some instances the cats
were etherized, in others they were anesthetized with urethane, or were
decerebrated. The splanchnic nerves always were stimulated after being
cut away from connection with, the spinal cord. Sometimes the nerves
were isolated unilaterally in the abdomen; sometimes, in order to avoid
manipulation of the abdominal viscera, they were isolated in the thorax
and stimulated singly or together, A tetanizing current was used,
barely perceptible on the tongue and too weak to cause by spreading any
contraction
of skeletal muscles.
COAGULATION HASTENED BY SPLANCHNIC STIMULATION
That splanchnic stimulation accelerates the clotting of
blood, and that the effects vary in different animals, are facts
illustrated in the following cases:
Oct. 25. A cat was etherized and maintained in uniform ether
anesthesia. After forty minutes of preliminary observation the left
splanchnic nerves were stimulated in the abdomen. Following are the
figures which show the effects on the coagulation time:
In this instance at least ten minutes elapsed between the end
of stimulation and the beginning of faster clotting. The period of
faster clotting, however, lasted for about a half-hour, during which
the coagulation time averaged 2.1 minutes, only fortythree per cent of
the previous average of 4.8 minutes.
It is noteworthy that the curve (see Fig. 29),
while lower, shows oscillations not unlike those which follow
injection of adrenin (see p. 155).
The primary delay of the effect is not always, indeed it is
not commonly, present:
Nov. 6. A cat was anesthetized (1.40 p.m.) with urethane, and
later (3.05) its brain was pithed. The following observations on the
coagulation time show the prompt effect of splanchnic stimulation:
In Fig. 30 is presented the original record of the shortening
of the coagulation after stimulation of the left splanchnic nerve (Nov.
8) in a cat with brain pithed. In the foregoing instances the
coagulation time was reduced after splanchnic stimulation to less than
half what it was before. The reduction was not always so pronounced.
Nov. 7. A cat* maintained in uniform ether anesthesia with
artificial respiration had the following changes in the clotting time
of its blood as the result of stimulating the left splanchnic nerve in
the thorax:
In this case the average for about fifteen minutes before
stimulation was slightly over five minutes,
* The animal had just passed through a period of
excitement
with rapid clotting.
and for twenty-five minutes thereafter it was four minutes.
In all cases thus far the period of shortened coagulation
lasted from ten to thirty minutes. In other cases, however, the effect
was seen only in a single observation. If this had occurred only once
after splanchnic stimulation, it might be attributed to accident, but
it was not an infrequent result, e. g.:
Oct. 28. A cat was etherized and decerebrated, and the
splanchnic nerves were isolated in the thorax. Following are two
instances of brief shortening of coagulation after splanchnic
stimulation:
In the foregoing instance it is noteworthy that the degree of
acceleration is not so great after the second stimulation of the
splanchnics as it was after the first. This reduction of effect as the
nerves were repeatedly stimulated was frequently noted. The following
case presents another illustration:
Nov. 12. A cat was etherized (2.35 p.m.) and the medulla was
punctured (piqûe) at 3.12. The operation was without effect. The
loss
or lessening of effectiveness on second stimulation of the left
splanchnic nerves is to be compared with the persistence of
effectiveness on the right side:
The experiments above recorded show that stimulation of the
splanchnic nerves results immediately, or after a brief period, in a
shortening of the coagulation time of the blood an effect which in
different animals varies in duration and intensity, and diminishes as
the stimulation is repeated. The next question was whether this effect
is produced through the adrenal glands.
COAGULATION NOT HASTENED BY SPLANCHNIC STIMULATION IF THE
ADRENAL GLANDS ARE ABSENT
The manner in which splanchnic stimulation produces its
effects is indicated in the following experiments:
Nov. 28. A cat was etherized, and through the orbit the
central nervous system was destroyed to the midthorax. The blood
vessels of the left adrenal gland were then quickly tied and the gland
removed. The readings for a half hour before the left splanchnic nerve
was stimulated averaged seven minutes, then -
Dec. 4. A cat was etherized and pithed through the orbit to
the neck region. The right and left splanchnic nerves were tied and cut
in the thorax. The left adrenal gland was then carefully
removed. These operations consumed about a half-hour. The following
records show the effect of stimulating the left and right splanchnic
nerves:
The results in this experiment are represented graphically in
Fig. 31.
Elliott's evidence that in the cat the splanchnic innervation
of the adrenals is not crossed has already been mentioned. If the gland
is removed on one side, therefore, stimulation of the nerves on that
side causes no discharge from the opposite gland. As the above
experiments clearly show, splanchnic stimulation on the glandless side
results in no shortening of the coagulation time; whereas, in the same
animals, stimulation of the nerves on the other side (still connected
with the adrenal gland) produces a sharp hastening of the clotting
process.
The splanchnics innervate the intestines and liver even
though the adrenal gland is removed. The foregoing experiments indicate
that the nerve impulses delivered to these organs do not influence them
in any direct manner to accelerate the speed of coagulation. Indeed, in
one of the experiments (Dec. 4, see Fig. 31) a high reading about ten
minutes after splanchnic stimulation on the glandless side suggests the
possibility of an opposite effect. Direct stimulation of the hepatic
nerves on one occasion was followed by a change of the clotting time
from 4.5, 5, 4.5, 4.5 minutes during twenty-five minutes before
stimulation to 4.5, 7, and 6 minutes during twenty minutes after
stimulation.
Since with the adrenals present stimulation of hepatic nerves
induces alteration of glycogen in the liver and quick increase of blood
sugar, 2 just as splanchnic stimulation does, the failure of the
blood to clot faster after stimulation of the hepatic nerves confirms
the evidence already offered that faster clotting when adrenin is
increased in the blood is not due to a larger amount of sugar present
(see p. 159).
The liver and intestines cannot be made to shorten clotting
time by stimulation of their nerves, but, as has already been shown
(see p. 157), neither can adrenin act by itself to hasten the clotting
process. Apparently the effect is produced by cooperation between the
adrenals and the liver (and possibly also the intestines) . Somewhat
similar cooperation is noted in the organization of sugar metabolism;
splanchnic stimulation in the absence of the adrenal glands does not
increase blood sugar, 3 and in the absence of the liver adrenin
is without influence. 4
The variations of effect noted after splanchnic stimulation
can be accounted for by variations in the adrenin content of the
glands. Elliott 5 found, as previously stated, that animals newly
brought into strange surroundings may have a considerably reduced
amount of adrenin in their adrenals. The animals used in our
experiments had been for varying lengths of time in an animal house in
which barking dogs were also kept, and were therefore subject to
influences which would be likely to discharge the glands.
The evidence that stimulation of splanchnic nerves, with
accompanying increase of adrenal secretion, results in more rapid
clotting of blood is especially interesting in relation to the
experiments previously described, which showed that in pain and
emotional excitement there is an increased secretion of adrenin into
the blood. Does the adrenin thus liberated have any effect on the rate
of coagulation ! The observations here recorded were made in order to
obtain an answer to that question.
COAGULATION HASTENED BY "PAINFUL" STIMULATION
In the experiments on the action of stimuli which in the
unanesthetized animal would cause pain, it will be recalled that
faradic stimulation of a large nerve trunk (the stump of the cut
sciatic)
and operation under light anesthesia were the methods used to
affect the afferent nerves. Elliott 6 found that repeated excitation of
the sciatic nerve was especially efficient in exhausting the adrenal
glands of their adrenin content, and also that this reflex persisted
after removal of the cerebral hemispheres. It was to be expected,
therefore, that with well-stored glands, sciatic stimulation, even in
the decerebrate animal, would call forth an amount of adrenal secretion
which would decidedly hasten clotting. The following case illustrates
such a result:
Dec. 12. A cat was anesthetized with ether at 3.45 and the
left sciatic nerve was bared. Decerebration
was completed at 3.57. The clotting time of the blood began
to be tested six minutes later:
The results obtained in this case, which were similar to
results in other cases, are represented graphically in Fig. 32. The
coagulation time was becoming gradually more prolonged, but each
excitation of the sciatic nerve was followed by a marked shortening.
The strength of stimulation was not determined with exactness, but it
is worthy of note that the current used in the first and the third
stimulations was weaker than could be felt on the tongue, whereas that
used in the second was considerably stronger, though it did not produce
reflex spasms.
Mere tying of the nerve is capable of producing a marked
shortening of coagulation, as the following figures show:
Oct. 21. 10.57 cat under ether, and urethane given:
Stimulation of the crural nerve had similar effects, reducing
the clotting time in one instance from a succession of 3, 3, and 3.5
minutes to 1.5 minutes shortly after the application of the current,
with a return to 3.5 minutes at the next test.
Operative procedures performed under light anaesthesia (i.
e., with the more persistent reflexes still present), or reduction of
anesthesia soon after operation, resulted in a remarkable shortening of
the coagulation time:
Nov. 8. A cat was etherized and tracheotomized. The abdomen
was then opened and a ligature was drawn around the hepatic nerves. The
operation was completed at 2.25. At 2.50 the etherization became light
and the rate of clotting began to be faster:
Nov. 11. A female cat, very quiet, was placed in the holder
at 1.55. The animal was not excited. At 2.10 etherization was begun;
the animal was then tracheotomized, and the femoral artery was exposed.
The results of this experiment are shown graphically in Fig.
33.
Nov. 13. A cat was etherized at 1.55, tracheotomized, and the
femoral artery laid bare. As soon as these preparations were completed,
the ether was removed and anesthesia became light. The blood clotted
thus:
In the foregoing and in other similar instances, a condition
of surgical injury, whether just made
or being made, was accompanied by more rapid clotting of
blood when the degree of anesthesia was
lessened. This condition was one which, if allowed to go
further in the same direction, would result in pain. Both direct
electrical stimulation and also surgical operation of a nature to give
pain in the unanesthetized animal result, therefore, in faster
clotting.
It is worthy of note that after decerebration clotting
apparently occurred no faster because the abdomen had been opened,
although in the deeerebrate state etherization was suspended. The
mechanism for reflex control of the adrenals may not be higher than the
corpora quadrigemina, as Elliott has shown, but the discharge from the
glands seems to be more certain to occur when the cerebrum is present
and is permitted even slightly to operate.
COAGULATION HASTENED IN EMOTIONAL EXCITEMENT
The evidence for emotional secretion of the adrenal glands
has already been presented. As was noted in my earlier observations on
the motions of the alimentary canal (see p. 14), cats differ widely in
their emotional reaction to being bound; some, especially young males,
become furious; others, especially elderly females, take the experience
quite calmly. This difference of attitude was used with positive
results, the reader will recall, in the experiments on emotional
glycosuria; there seemed a possibility likewise of using it to test the
effect of emotions on blood clotting. To plan formal experiments for
that purpose was not necessary, because in the ordinary course of the
researches here reported, the difference in effects on the blood
between
the violent rage of vigorous young males and the quiet
complacency of old females was early noted. Indeed, the rapid clotting
which accompanied excitement not infrequently made necessary an
annoying wait till slower clotting would permit the use of experimental
methods for shortening the process.
The animals used on November 11 and 13 (see pp. 175, 176) are
examples of calm acceptance of being placed on the holder; and
furthermore, these animals were anesthetized without much disturbance.
As the figures indicate, the clotting from the first occurred at about
the average rate.
In sharp contrast to these figures are those obtained when a
vigorous animal is angered:
Oct. 30. A very vigorous cat was placed on the holder at
9.08. It at once became stormy, snarling, hissing, biting, and lashing
its big tail. At 9.12 etherizing was begun and that intensified the
excitement. By 9.15 the femoral artery was tied. The clotting time of
the blood for an hour after the ether was first given was as follows:
Twenty-four observations made during the hour showed that the
clotting time in this enraged animal averaged three-fourths of a minute
and was never longer than a minute and a half. The clots were
invariably a solid jelly. The persistence of the rapid clotting for so
long a period after anesthesia was started may have been in part due to
continued, rather light, etherization, for Elliott 7 found that
etherization itself could reduce the adrenin content of the adrenal
glands.
The shortened clotting did not always persist so long as in
the foregoing instance. The brief period of faster clotting illustrated
in the following case was typical of many:
Nov. 18. A cat that had been in stock for some time was
placed on the holder at 2.13, and was at once enraged. Two minutes
later etherization was started. The hairs on the tail were erect. The
clotting was as follows:
It seems probable that in this case just as in some of the
cases in which the splanchnic nerves were stimulated (see p. 166), the
adrenals had been well-nigh exhausted because of the cat's being caged
near dogs, and that the emotional flare-up practically discharged the
glands, for repeated at tempts later to reproduce the initial rapid
clotting by stimulation of the splanchnic nerves were without result.
Evidence presented in previous chapters makes wholly probable
the correctness of the inference that the faster coagulation which
follows emotional excitement is due to adrenal discharge from
splanchnic stimulation. In this relation the effect of severance of the
splanchnics on emotional acceleration of the clotting process is of
interest. The following cases are illustrative:
Oct. 29. A cat was left on the holder for ten minutes while
the femoral artery was uncovered under local anesthesia. The blood
removed was clotted in a half-minute. The animal was much excited. It
was now quickly etherized and the brain pithed forward from the neck.
The tests resulted as follows:
The original record of this case is given in Fig. 34.
Nov. 5. A cat was etherized at 2.35. At 2.39 artificial
respiration by tracheal cannula was begun, the air passing through an
ether bottle. The clotting occurred thus:
Nov. 7. A cat was etherized at 1.55 under excitement and with
tail hairs erect. At 2.13 the animal
was showing reflexes. The figures show the course of the
experiment:
In this instance the subsequent stimulation of the splanchnic
nerves resulted again in faster clotting a reduction from 5.5 minutes
to 3.5 minutes (see experiment Nov. 7, p. 164). The results from this
experiment are expressed graphically in Fig. 35.
The data presented in this chapter show that such stimulation
as in the unanesthetized animal would cause pain, and also such
emotions as fear and rage, are capable of greatly shortening the
coagulation time of blood. These results are quite in harmony with the
evidence previously offered that injected adrenin and secretion from
the adrenal glands induced by splanchnic stimulation hasten clotting,
for painful stimulation and emotional excitement also evoke activity of
the adrenals. Here, then, is another fundamental change
in the body, a change tending to the conservation of its most
important fluid, wrought through the adrenal glands in times of great
perturbation. This bodily change and the others which occur under the
same circumstances are next to be examined with reference to their
significance.
REFERENCES
1 Cannon and Mendenhall: American Journal of Physiology,
1914, xxxiv, p. 251.
2 Macleod: Diabetes: its Pathological Physiology, London,
1913, pp. 68-72.
8 Gautrelet and Thomas: Comptes Rendus, Societe de Biologie,
1909, Ixvii, p. 233.
4 Bang: Der Blutzucker, Wiesbaden, 1913, p. 87.
5 Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xliv, p. 379.
6 Elliott: Loc. cit., pp. 406, 407.
7 Elliott: Loc. cit., p. 388.
CHAPTER XI
THE UTILITY OF THE BODILY CHANGES IN PAIN AND GREAT EMOTION
We now turn from a consideration of the data secured in our
experiments to an interpretation of the data. One of the most important
lessons of experience is learning to distinguish between the facts of
observation and the inferences drawn from those facts. The facts may
remain unquestioned; the explanation, however, may be changed by
additional facts or through the influence of more extensive views.
Having given this warning, I propose to discuss the bearings of the
results reported in the earlier chapters.
Our inquiry thus far has revealed that the adrenin secreted
by the adrenal glands in times of stress has all the effects in the
body that are produced by injected adrenin. It plays an essential role
in calling forth stored carbohydrate from the liver, thus flooding the
blood with sugar; it helps in distributing the blood to the heart,
lungs, central nervous system and limbs, while taking it away from the
inhibited organs of the abdomen; it
quickly abolishes the effects of muscular fatigue; and it
renders the blood more rapidly coagulable. These remarkable facts are,
furthermore, associated with some of the most primitive experiences in
the life of higher organisms, experiences common to all, both man and
beast the elemental experiences of pain and fear and rage that come
suddenly in critical emergencies. What is the significance of these
profound bodily alterations? What are the emergency functions of
secreted adrenin?
THE REFLEX NATURE OF BODILY RESPONSES IN PAIN AND THE MAJOR
EMOTIONS, AND THE USEFUL CHARACTER OF REFLEXES
The most significant feature of these bodily reactions in
pain and in the presence of emotion-provoking
objects is that they are of the nature of reflexes they are
not willed movements, indeed they are often distressingly beyond the
control of the will. The pattern of the reaction, in these as in other
reflexes, is deeply inwrought in the workings of the nervous system,
and when the appropriate occasion arises, typical organic responses are
evoked through inherent automatisms.
It has long been recognized that the most characteristic
feature of reflexes is their "purposive" nature, or their utility
either in preserving the welfare of the organism or in safeguarding it
against injury. The reflexes of sucking, swallowing, vomiting and
coughing, for instance, need only to be mentioned to indicate the
variety of ways in which reflexes favor the continuance of existence.
When, therefore, these automatic responses accompanying pain and fear
and rage the increased discharge of adrenin and sugar are
under consideration, it is reasonable to inquire first as to
their utility.
Numerous ingenious suggestions have been offered to account
for the more obvious changes accompanying emotional states as, for
example, the terrifying aspect produced by the bristling of the hair
and the uncovering of the teeth in an access of rage. 1 The most widely
applicable explanation proposed for these spontaneous reactions is that
during the long course of racial experience they have been developed
for quick
service in the struggle for existence. Earlier writers on
organic evolution pointed out the anticipatory character of these
responses. According to Spencer, 2 "Fear, when strong, expresses itself
in cries, in efforts to hide or escape, in palpitations and tremblings;
and these are just the manifestations that would accompany an actual
experience of the evil feared. The destructive passions are shown in a
general tension of the muscular system, in gnashing of the teeth and
protrusion of the claws, in dilated eyes and nostrils, in growls; and
these are weaker forms of the actions that accompany the killing of
prey." McDougall 3 has developed this idea systematically and has
suggested that an association has become established between
peculiar emotions and peculiar instinctive reactions; thus
the emotion of fear is associated with the instinct for flight, and the
emotion of anger or rage with the instinct for fighting or attack.
Crile 4 likewise in giving recent expression to the same view has
emphasized the importance of adaptation and natural selection,
operative through myriads of years of racial experience, in enabling us
to account for the already channeled
responses which we find established in our nervous
organization. And on a principle of "phylogenetic association" he
assumes that fear, born of innumerable injuries in the course of
evolution, has developed into portentous foreshadowing of possible
injury and has become, therefore, capable of arousing in the body all
the offensive and defensive activities that favor the survival of the
organism.
Because the increase of adrenin and the increase of sugar in
the blood, following painful or strong emotional experiences, are
reflex in character, and because reflexes as a rule are useful
responses, we are justified in the assumption that under these
circumstances these reactions are useful. What, then, is their possible
value?
In order that these reactions may be useful they must be
prompt. Such is the case. Some observations made by one of my students,
Mr. H. Osgood, show that the latent period of adrenal secretion, when
the splanchnic nerve is stimulated below the diaphragm, is not longer
than 16 seconds; and Macleod 5 states that within a few minutes after
splanchnic stimulation the sugar in the blood rises between 10 and 30
per cent. The two
secretions are, therefore, almost instantly ready for
service.
Conceivably the two secretions might act in conjunction, or
each might have its own function alone. Thus adrenin might serve in
cooperation with nervous excitement to produce increase of blood sugar,
or it might have that function and other functions quite apart from
that. Before these possibilities are considered, however, the value of
the increased blood sugar itself will be discussed.
THE UTILITY OF THE INCREASED BLOOD SUGAR AS A SOURCE OF
MUSCULAR ENERGY
When we were working on emotional glycosuria a clue to the
significance of the increase of sugar in the blood was found in
McDougall's suggestion of a relation between "flight instinct" and
"fear emotion," and "pugnacity instinct" and "anger emotion." And the
point was made that, since the fear emotion and the anger emotion are,
in wild life, likely to be followed by activities (running or fighting)
which require contraction of great muscular masses in supreme and
prolonged struggle, a mobilization of sugar in the blood might be
of signal service to the laboring muscles. Pain and fighting
is almost certain to involve pain would, if possible, call forth even
greater muscular effort. "In the agony of pain almost every muscle of
the body is brought into strong action" Darwin 6 wrote, for
"great pain urges all animals, and has urged them during endless
generations, to make the most violent and diversified efforts to escape
from the cause of suffering."*
* It is recognized that both pain and the major
emotions may
have at times depressive rather than stimulating" effects. For example,
Martin and Lacey have shown (American Journal of Physiology, 1014,
xxxiii, p. 212) that such stimuli as would induce pain may cause a fall
of blood pressure, and they suggest that the rise of blood pressure
commonly reported at times of painful experience is due to the psychic
disturbance that is simultaneously aroused. Conceivably there is a
relation between recognizing the possibility of escape (with the
psychic consequences of that possibility) and the degree of stimulating
effect. Thus pains originating from the interior of the body, or from
injuries sure to be made more painful by action, would not likely lead
to action. On the other hand, the whip and spur illustrate the
well-known excitant effect of painful stimuli. Similarly in the case of
the strong emotions, the effect may be paralyzing until there is a definite
deed to perform. Thus terror may be the most depressing of all
emotions, but, as Darwin pointed out (Loc. cit., fp. 81), "a man or
animal driven through terror to desperation is endowed with wonderful
strength, and is notoriously dangerous in the highest
degree."
That muscular work is performed by energy supplied in
carbonaceous material is shown by the great increase of carbon-dioxide
output in severe muscular work, which may exceed twenty times the
output during rest. Furthermore, the storage of glycogen in muscle, and
the disappearance of this glycogen deposit from excised muscle
stimulated to activity, 7 or its reduction after excessive contractions
produced by strychnine,8 and the lessened ability of muscles to work if
their glycogen store has been reduced, 9 and the simple chemical
relation between sugar and the lactic acid which appears when muscles
are repeatedly made
to contract, are all indications that carbohydrate (sugar and
glycogen) is the elective source of energy for contraction. This
conclusion is supported in recent careful studies by Benedict and
Cathcart, 10 who have shown that a small but distinct increase in the
ratio between the carbondioxide breathed out and the oxygen breathed in
during a given period (the respiratory quotient) occurs during muscular
work, and that a decrease
in the quotient follows, thus pointing to a larger proportion
of carbohydrate burned during muscular work than before or after i. e.,
a call on the carbohydrate deposits of the body.
Whether circulating sugar can be immediately utilized by
active muscles has been a subject of dispute.
The claim of Chauveau and Kaufmanu 11 that a muscle uses
about three and a half times as much blood sugar when active as when
resting, although supported by Quinquaud, 12 and by Morat and Dufourt,
13 has been denied by Pavy, 14 who failed to find any difference
between the sugar content of arterial and venous blood when the muscle
was contracting; and also by Magnus-Levy, 15 who has estimated that the
amount of change in sugar content of the blood passing through a muscle
must be so slight as to be within the limits of the error of analysis.
On the other hand, when blood or Ringer's solution is repeatedly
perfused through contracting heart muscle, the evidence is clear that
the contained sugar may more or less completely disappear.
Thus Locke and Rosenheim 16 found that from 5 to 10
centigrams of dextrose disappeared from Ringer's solution repeatedly
circulated through the rabbit heart for eight or nine hours. And
recently Patterson and Starling 17 have shown that if blood is perfused
repeatedly through a heart-lung preparation for three or four hours,
and the heart is continually stimulated by adrenin added to the blood,
the sugar in the blood wholly
vanishes; or if the supply of sugar is maintained, the
consumption may rise as high as 8 milligrams per gram of heart muscle
per hour about four times the usual consumption. When an animal is
eviscerated it may be regarded as a preparation in which the muscles
are perfused with their proper blood, pumped by the heart and
oxygenated by the lungs. Under these circumstances, the percentage of
sugar in the blood steadily falls, 18 because the utilization by the
tissues is not compensated for by further supply from the liver. Thus,
although there may be doubt that analyses of sugar in the blood flowing
into and out from an active muscle during a brief period can be
accurate enough to prove a clear difference, the evidence from the
experiments above cited shows that when the supply of sugar is limited
it disappears to a greater or less degree if passed repeatedly through
muscular organs.
The argument may be advanced, of course, that the sugar which
thus disappears is not directly utilized, but must first be changed to
glycogen. There is little basis for this assumption. There is, on the
other hand, considerable evidence that increasing the blood sugar does,
in fact, directly increase muscular efficiency. Thus Locke 19 proved
that if oxygenated salt solution is perfused through the isolated
rabbit heart, the beats begin to weaken after one or two hours; but if
now 0.1 per cent dextrose is added to the perfusing liquid, the beats
at once become markedly stronger and may continue with very slow
lessening of strength as long as seven hours. And Schumberg 20 noted
that when he performed a large amount of general bodily work (thus
using up blood sugar) and then tested flexion of the middle finger in
an ergograph, the ability of the muscle was greater if he drank a sugar
solution than if he drank an equally sweet solution of "dulcin." He did
not
know during the experiment which solution he was drinking.
These observations have been confirmed by Prantner and Stowasser, and
by Frentzel. 21 In experiments on cats, Lee and Harrold 22 found that
when sugar is removed from the animal by means of phlorhizin the tibialis
anticus is quickly fatigued; but if, after the phlorhizin
treatment, the animal is given an abundance of sugar and then submitted
to the test, the muscle shows a much larger capacity for work. All this
evidence is, of course, favorable to the view that circulating
sugar may be quickly utilized by contracting muscles.
From the experimental results presented above it is clear
that muscles work preferably by utilizing the energy stored in sugar,
that great muscular labor is capable of considerably reducing the
quantity of stored glycogen and of circulating sugar, and that under
circumstances of a lessened sugar content the increase of blood sugar
considerably augments the ability of muscles to continue contracting.
The conclusion seems justified, therefore, that the increased blood
sugar attendant on the major emotions and pain would be of direct
benefit to the organism in the strenuous muscular efforts involved in
flight or conflict or struggle to
be free.
THE UTILITY OF INCREASED ADRENIN IN THE BLOOD AS AN ANTIDOTE
TO THE EFFECTS OF FATIGUE
The function which the discharged adrenin itself might have
in favoring vigorous muscular contraction has already been suggested in
the chapter on the effect of adrenin in restoring the irritability of
fatigued muscle. Some of the earliest evidence proved that removal of
the adrenal glands has a debilitating effect on muscular power, and
that injection of adrenal extract has an invigorating effect. For these
reasons it seemed possible that increased adrenal secretion, as a
reflex result of pain or the major emotions, might act in itself as a
dynamogenic factor in the performance of muscular work. It was on the
basis of that possibility that Nice and I tested the effect of
stimulating the splanchnic nerves (thus causing adrenal secretion), or
injecting adrenin, on the contraction of the fatigued tibialis anticus.
We found, as already described, that when arterial pressure was of
normal height, and was prevented from rising in the legs while the
splanchnic was being stimulated, there was a distinct rise in the
height of contraction of the fatigued muscle. And we drew the
inference that adrenin set free in the blood may operate
favorably to the organism by preparing fatigued muscles for better
response to the nervous discharges sent forth in great excitement.
This inference led to the experiments by Gruber, who examined
the effects of minute amounts of adrenin (0.1 or 0.5 cubic centimeter,
1:100,000), and also of splanchnic stimulation, on the threshold
stimulus of fatigued neuro-muscular and muscular apparatus. Fatigue,
the reader will recall, raises the threshold not uncommonly 100 or 200
per cent, and in some instances as much as 600 per cent. Best will
restore the normal threshold in periods varying from fifteen minutes to
two hours, according to the length of previous stimulation. If a small
dose of adrenin is given, however, the normal threshold may be restored
in three to five minutes.
From the foregoing evidence the conclusion is warranted that
adrenin, when freely liberated in the blood, not only aids in bringing
out sugar from the liver's store of glycogen, but also has a remarkable
influence in quickly restoring to fatigued muscles, which have lost
their original irritability, the same readiness for response which they
had when fresh. Thus the adrenin set free in pain and in fear and rage
would put the muscles of the body unqualifiedly at the disposal of the
nervous system; the difficulty which nerve impulses might have in
calling the muscles into full activity would be practically abolished;
and this provision, along
with the abundance of energy-supplying sugar newly flushed
into the circulation, would give to the animal in which these
mechanisms are most efficient the best possible conditions for putting
forth supreme muscular efforts.*
THE QUESTION WHETHER ADRENIN NORMALLY SECRETED INHIBITS THE
USE OF SUGAR IN THE BODY
The only evidence opposed to the conclusion which has just
been drawn is that which may be found in results which were noted by
Wilenko. He injected adrenin into urethanized rabbits, usually one
milligram per kilo body weight, and then found that the animals did not
oxidize any part of an intravenous injection of glucose. Babbits
supplied with glucose in a similar manner, but not given adrenin, have
an increased respiratory quotient. Wilenko 23 concluded, therefore,
that adrenin lessens the capacity of the organism to burn
carbohydrates. In a later paper he reported that adrenin, when added,
with glucose, to physiological salt
solution (Locke's), and perfused through the isolated rabbit
heart, notably increases the use of sugar by the heart (from 2.2-2.8 to
2.9-4.3 milligrams
* If these results of emotion and pain are not "worked
off"
by action, it is conceivable that the excessive adrenin and sugar in
the blood may have pathological effects. (Cf. Cannon: Journal of the
American Medical Association, 1911, Ivi, p. 742.)
of glucose per gram of heart muscle per hour), but that the
heart removed after the animal has received a subcutaneous injection of
adrenin uses much less sugar, only 0.5-1.2 milligrams per gram per
hour. From these results Wilenko 24 concludes that the glycosuria
following injection of adrenin is the result of disturbance of the use
of sugar an effect which is not direct on the sugar-consuming organ,
but indirect through action on some other organ.
Wilenko's conclusion fails to account readily for the
disappearance of glycogen from the liver in adrenin glycosuria.
Furthermore, Lusk 25 has recently reported that the subcutaneous
administration of adrenin (one milligram per kilo body weight) to dogs,
simultaneously with 50 grams of glucose by mouth, interferes not at all
with the use of the sugar the respiratory quotient remains for several
hours at 1.0; i. e., at the figure which glucose alone would have
given. In other words, Lusk's results with dogs are directly
contradictory
to Wilenko's results with rabbits. Nevertheless, Wilenko's
conclusion might be quite true for the glycosuria produced by adrenin
alone (which must be excessive), and yet have no bearing whatever on
the glycosuria produced physiologically by splanchnic stimulation, even
though some adrenin is thereby simultaneously liberated.
The amount of injected adrenin used to produce adrenin
glycosuria is enormous. Osgood has studied
in the Harvard Physiological Laboratory the effects on blood
pressure of alternately stimulating the left splanchnic nerves (with
the splanchnic vessels eliminated) and injecting adrenin, and by this
method of comparison 26 has shown that the amount secreted after five
seconds of stimulation varies between 0.0015 and 0.007 milligram. If
0.005 milligram is taken as a rather high average figure, and doubled
(for two glands), the amount would be 0.01 milligram. To produce
adrenin glycosuria, an animal weighing two kilos would be injected with
two hundred times this amount. It is granted that more adrenin would be
secreted if
the nerves were stimulated longer than five seconds, and that
with injection under the skin or into the abdominal cavity (to produce
glycosuria), the amount of adrenin in the blood at one time would not
be so great as if the injection were into a vein; but even with these
concessions the amount of adrenin in the blood, when it has been
injected to produce glycosuria, is probably very much above the amount
following physiological stimulation of the glands.
Other evidence that the amount of adrenin discharged when the
glands are stimulated is not so great as the amount needed to produce
glycosuria when acting alone is presented in experiments by Macleod .27
He found that if the nerve fibres to the liver were destroyed,
stimulation of the splanchnic, which would cause increased adrenal
secretion, did not increase the blood sugar. The increased blood sugar
due to splanchnic stimulation, therefore, is a nervous effect,
dependent, to be sure, on the presence of adrenin in the blood,
but the amount of adrenin present is not in itself capable of
evoking increase.
Furthermore, the increased blood sugar following splanchnic
stimulation may long outlast the stimulation period. The adrenals,
however, as has been demonstrated by Osgood, are soon fatigued, and
fail to respond to repeated stimulation. They seem to be incapable of
prolonged action.
Again, as Macleod 28 has shown, a rise in the sugar content
of the blood can be induced, if the adrenals are intact, merely by
stimulating the nerves going to the liver. The increased blood sugar of
splanchnic origin, therefore, is not due to a disturbance of the use of
sugar in the body, as Wilenko claims for the increase after adrenin
injection, but is a result of a breaking down of the stored glycogen in
the liver and is of nervous
origin.
We may conclude, therefore, that since the conditions of
Wilenko's observations are not comparable with emotional conditions,
his inferences are not pertinent to the present discussion; that when
both adrenin and sugar are increased in the blood as a result of
excitement, the higher percentage of sugar is not due to adrenin
inhibiting the use of sugar by the tissues, and that there is no
evidence at present to show that the brief augmentation of adrenal
discharge, following excitement or splanchnic stimulation, affects in
any deleterious manner the utilization of sugar as a source of energy.
Indeed, the observation of Wilenko and of Patterson and
Starling, above mentioned, that adrenin increases the use of sugar by
the heart, may signify that a physiological discharge of the adrenals
can have a favorable rather than an unfavorable effect on the
employment of sugar by the tissues.
THE VASCULAR CHANGES PRODUCED BY ADRENIN FAVORABLE TO SUPREME
MUSCULAR EXERTION
Quite in harmony with the foregoing argument that sugar and
adrenin, which are poured into the blood during emotional excitement,
render the organism more efficient in the physical struggle for
existence, are the vascular changes wrought by increased adrenin,
probably in cooperation with sympathetic innervations. The studies of
volume changes of parts of the body, by Oliver and Schaefer and others,
have already been noted. Their observations, it will be remembered,
showed that injected adrenin drove the blood from the abdominal viscera
into the organs called upon in emergencies into the central nervous
system, the lungs, the heart, and the active skeletal muscles. The
absence of effective vasoconstrictor nerves in the brain and the lungs,
and the dilation of vessels in the heart and skeletal muscles during
times of increased activity, make the blood supply to these parts
dependent on the height of general arterial pressure. In pain and great
excitement, as we have already seen, this pressure is likely to be much
elevated, and consequently the blood
flow through the unconstricted or actually dilated vessels of
the body will be all the more abundant.
Adrenin has a well-known stimulating effect on the isolated
heart causing an increase both in the rate and the amplitude of cardiac
contraction. This effect accords with the general rule that adrenin
simulates the action of sympathetic impulses. It is commonly stated,
however, that if the heart holds its normal relations in the body,
adrenin causes slowing of the beat. 29 This view is doubtless due to
the massive doses that have been employed, which are quite beyond
physiological limits and which induce such enormous increases of
arterial pressure that the natural influence of adrenin on heart muscle
is overcome by mechanical obstacles to quick contractions and by
inhibitory impulses from the central nervous system. Hoskins and
Lovellette have recently shown that when the precaution is taken to
inject adrenin into a vein in a manner resembling the discharge from
the adrenal glands, not only is there increased blood pressure, but
generally, also, an acceleration of the pulse. 30 At the same time,
therefore, that a greater amount of work, from increased arterial
pressure, is demanded of the heart, blood is delivered to the heart in
greater abundance, and the
muscle is excited to more rapid and vigorous pulsations. The
augmentation of the heart beat is thus coordinate with the other
adaptive functions of the adrenal glands in great emergencies.
THE CHANGES IN RESPIRATORY FUNCTON ALSO FAVORABLE TO GREAT
EFFORT
The urgent need in struggle or flight is a generous supply of
oxygen to oxidize the metabolites of muscular contraction, and a quick
riddance of the resultant carbon-dioxide from the body. The moment
vigorous exercise is begun the breathing at once changes so as to bring
about a more thorough ventilation of the lungs. And one of the most
characteristic reactions of animals in pain and emotional excitement is
deep and rapid respiration. Again the reflex response is precisely what
would be most serviceable to the organism in the strenuous efforts of
fighting or escape that might accompany or follow distress or fear or
rage. It is
known that by such forced respirations the carbondioxide
content of the blood can be so much reduced that the need for any
breathing whatever may be deferred for as much as a minute or even
longer. 31 And Douglas and Haldane 32 have found that moderately forced
breathing for three minutes previous to severe muscular exertion
results in greatly diminishing the subsequent respiratory distress, as
well as lessening the amount of air breathed and the amount of
carbon-dioxide given off. Furthermore, the heart beats less rapidly
after the performance and returns more quickly from its
increased rate to normal. The forced respirations in deeply emotional
experiences can be interpreted, therefore, as an anticipatory reduction
of the carbon-dioxide in the blood, a preparation for the augmented
discharge of carbon-dioxide into the blood as soon as great muscular
exertion begins.*
As the air moves to and fro in the lungs with each
respiration, it must pass through- the fine divisions of the air tubes
or bronchioles. The bronchioles are provided with smooth muscle, which,
in all probability, like smooth muscle elsewhere in the body, is
normally held in a state of
* The excessive production of heat in muscular work
gives
rise to sweating. The evaporation of sweat helps to keep the body
temperature from rising unduly from the heat of exertion. Again in
strong emotion and in pain the "cold sweat" that appears on the skin
may be regarded as a reaction anticipatory of the strenuous muscular
movements that are likely to ensue.
Ionic contraction. When this tonic contraction is much
increased, as in asthma, breathing becomes difficult, and even with the
body at rest unusual effort is then required to maintain the minimal
necessary ventilation of the lungs. During strenuous exertion, with
each breath the air must rush through the bronchioles in greatly
increased volume and speed. Thus in a well person "winded" with
running, for example, the bronchioles might become relatively too small
for the stream of air, just as they are too small in a person ill with
asthma. And then some extra energy would have to be expended to force
the air back and forth with sufficient rapidity to satisfy the bodily
needs. It is probable that even under the most favorable conditions,
the labored breathing in hard exercise involves to some degree the work
of accelerating the tidal flow of the respiratory gases. This extra
labor would obviously be reduced, if the tonic contraction of the
ring-muscles in the wall of the bronchioles was reduced, so that the
tubules were enlarged. It has been shown by a number of investigators,
who have used various methods, that adrenin injected into the blood
stream has as one of its precise actions the dilating of the
bronchioles. 33 The adrenin discharged in emotional excitement goes to
the lungs before entering into relation with any other organ except the
right heart chamber; it may, therefore, have as its first effect the
relaxation of the smooth muscles of the lungs. This would be another
very direct means of rendering the organism more efficient when fierce
struggle calls for a bounteous supply of fresh
air and a speedy discharge of the carbonaceous waste:
EFFECTS PRODUCED IN ASPHYXIA SIMILAR TO THOSE PRODUCED IN
PAIN AND EXCITEMENT
All the bodily responses occurring in pain and emotional
excitement have thus far been considered as anticipatory of the
instinctive acts which naturally follow. And as we have seen, these
responses can reasonably be interpreted as preparatory to the great
exertions which may be demanded of the organism. This interpretation of
the facts is supported by the discovery that a mechanism exists whereby
the changed initiated in an anticipatory manner by emotional excitement
are continued or perhaps augumented by the
exertion itself.
Great exertion, such as might attend flight or conflict,
would result in an excessive production of carbon-dioxide. Then,
although respiratory and circulatory changes of emotional origin may
have prepared the body for struggle, the emotional provisions for
keeping the working parts at a high level of efficiency may not
continue to operate, or they may not be adequate. If there is painful
gasping for breath in the course of prolonged and vigorous exertion, or
for a considerable period after the work has ceased, a condition of
partial asphyxia has evidently been induced. This condition, as
everyone knows, is distinctly unfavorable
to further effort. But the asphyxia itself may act as a
stimulus. 34 In our examination of the influence of various conditions
on the secretion of the adrenal glands, Hoskins and I 35 tested the
effects of asphyxia.
By use of the intestinal segment as an indicator we compared
the action of blood, taken as nearly simultaneously as possible from
the vena cava above the adrenal vessels and from the femoral vein
before
asphyxia, with blood taken from the same sources after
asphyxia had been produced. The femoral venous blood after passing the
capillaries of the leg thus acted as a standard for the same blood
after receiving the contribution of the adrenal veins. Asphyxia was
caused by covering the trachea! cannula until respiration became
labored and slow, but capable of recovery when air was admitted. It may
be regarded, therefore, as
not extreme.
The results of the degree of asphyxia above described are
shown by graphic record in Fig. 36. Blood taken from the vena cava and
from the femoral vein before asphyxia ("normal") failed to cause
inhibition of the contractions. Blood taken from the femoral vein after
asphyxia produced almost the same effect as blood from the same vein
before; asphyxia, therefore, had wrought no change demonstrable in the
general venous flow.
Blood taken from the vena cava after asphyxia had, on the
contrary, an effect markedly unlike blood from the same region before
(compare the record after 1 and after 7, Fig. 36) it caused the typical
inhibition which indicates the presence of adrenal secretion.*
That the positive result obtained in moderate asphyxia is not
attributable to other agencies in the blood than adrenin is indicated
by the failure of asphyxial femoral blood to cause inhibition, while
vena-cava blood, taken almost simultaneously, brought about immediate
relaxation of the muscle. The conclusion was drawn, therefore, that
asphyxia results in increased secretion of the adrenal glands.
This conclusion has been supported by Anrep, 36 who noted
contraction of a denervated limb during asphyxia, though general
arterial pressure rose; and by Gasser and Meek, 37 who, while studying
a dog with denervated heart, found that when the
* This positive result might suggest that the
comparison of
both femoral and vena-cava blood under each condition was unnecessary,
and that a comparison merely of vena-cava blood before and after
asphyxia would be sufficient. Positive results were indeed thus
secured, but they occurred even when the adrenal glands were carefully
removed and extreme asphyxia (i. e., stoppage of respiration) was
induced. That the blood may contain in extreme asphyxia a substance or
substances capable of causing inhibition of intestinal contractions was
thus demonstrated. In one instance, after the blood was proved free
from adrenin, the aorta and vena cava were tied close below the
diaphragm, and the carotids were tied about midway in the neck. Extreme
asphyxia was produced (lasting five minutes). Blood now taken from the
heart caused marked inhibition of the beating intestinal segment.
Probably, therefore, the inhibitory action of blood taken from an
animal when extremely asphyxiated cannot be due to adrenin alone.
animal was asphyxiated, the pulse increased 90 beats per
minute. These effects were not seen after exclusion of the adrenal
glands. The observations on the denervated heart I have recently
confined. 38 Asphyxia, like pain and excitement, not only liberates
adrenin, but, as might be inferred from that fact, also mobilizes
sugar. 39 And, furthermore, Starkenstein 40 has shown that the asphyxia
due to carbon-monoxide poisoning is not accompanied by increased blood
sugar if the adrenal glands have been removed.
In case strong emotions are followed by vigorous exertions,
therefore, asphyxia is likely to result, and this will act in
conjunction with the emotional excitement and pain, or perhaps in
continuation of the influences of these states, to bring forth still
more adrenal discharge and still further output of sugar from the
liver. And these in turn would serve the laboring muscles in the manner
already described. This suggestion is in accord
with Macleod's 41 that the increased freeing of glycogen from
the liver produced by muscular exercise is possibly associated with
increased carbondioxide in the blood. And it also harmonizes with
Zuntz's statement 42 that the asphyxia of great physical exertion may
call out sugar to such a degree that, in spite of the increased use of
it in the Active muscles, glycosuria may ensue.
The evidence previously adduced that adrenin causes
relaxation of the smooth muscle of the bronchioles, taken in
conjunction with the evidence that adrenal secretion is liberated in
asphyxia, suggests that relief from difficult breathing may thus be
automatically provided for in the organism. The well-known phenomenon
of "second wind" is characterized by an almost miraculous refreshment
and renewal of vigor, after an individual has persisted in violent
exertion in spite of being "out of breath." It seems not improbable
that this phenomenon, for which many explanations have been offered, is
really due to setting in operation the supporting mechanism which, as
we have seen, plays so important a role in augmenting bodily
vigor in emotional excitement. The release of sugar and
adrenin, the abundance of blood flow through the muscles supplying
energy and lessening fatigue and the relaxation of the bronchiolar
walls, are all occurrences which may reasonably be regarded as
resulting from asphyxia. And when they take place they doubtless do
much to abolish the distress itself by which they were occasioned.
According to this explanation "second wind" would consist in the
establishment of the same group of bodily changes, leading to more
efficient physical struggle, that are observed in pain and excitement.
THE UTILITY OP RAPID COAGULATION IN PREVENTING LOSS OF BLOOD
The increase of blood sugar, the secretion of adrenin, and
the altered circulation in pain and emotional excitement have been
interpreted in the foregoing discussion as biological adaptations to
conditions in wild life which are likely to involve pain and emotional
excitement, i. e., the necessities of fighting or flight. The more
rapid clotting of blood under these same circumstances may also be
regarded as an adaptive process, useful to the organism. The importance
of conserving the blood, especially in the struggles of mortal combat,
needs no argument. The effect of local injury in favoring the formation
of a clot to seal the opened vessels is obviously adaptive in
protecting the organism against hemorrhage. The injury that causes
opening of blood vessels, however, is, if extensive, likely also to
produce pain. And, as already shown, conditions producing pain increase
adrenal secretion and hasten coagulation. Thus injury would be made
less dangerous as an occasion for serious hemorrhage by two effects
which the injury itself produces in the body the local effect on
clotting at the region of injury and the general effect on the 'speed
of clotting wrought by reflex
secretion of adrenin. According to the argument here
presented the strong emotions, as fear and anger, are rightly
interpreted as the concomitants of bodily changes which may be of
utmost service in subsequent action. These bodily changes are so much
like those which occur in pain and fierce struggle that, as early
writers on evolution suggested, the emotions may be considered as
foreshadowing the suffering and intensity of actual strife. On this
general basis, therefore, the bodily alterations attending violent
emotional states would, as organic preparations for fighting and
possible injury, naturally involve the effects which pain itself would
produce. And increased blood sugar, increased adrenin, an adapted
circulation and rapid clotting
would all be favorable to the preservation of the organism
that could best produce them.
REFERENCES
1 See Darwin: Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals, New
York, 1905, pp. 101, 117.
2 Spencer: Principles of Psychology, London, 1855.
3 McDougall: Introduction to Social Psychology, London, 1908,
pp. 49, 59.
4 Crile: Boston Medical and Surgical Journal, 1910, clxiii,
p. 893.
5 Macleod: Diabetes, etc., p. 80.
6 Darwin: Loc. cit. f p. 72.
7 Nasse: Archiv für die gesammte Physiologie, 1869, ii,
p.
106; 1877, xiv, p. 483.
8 Frentzel: Archiv für die gesammte Physiologie, 1894,
Ivi,
p. 280.
9 Zuntz: Oppenheimer's Handbuch der Biochemie, Jena, 1911, iv
(first half), p. 841.
10 Benedict and Cathcart: Muscular Work, a Metabolic Study,
Washington, 1913, pp. 85-87.
11 Chauveau and Kaufmann: Comptes Rendus, Academic des
Sciences, 1886, ciii, p. 1062.
12 Quinquaud: Comptes Rendus, Societé de Biologie,
1886,
xxxviii, p. 410.
13 Morat and Dufourt: Archives de Physiologie, 1892, xxiv, p.
327.
14 Pavy: The Physiology of the Carbohydrates, London, 1894,
p. 166.
15 Magnus-Levy: v. Noorden's Handbuch der Pathologie des
Stoffwechsels, 1906, i, p. 385.
16 Locke and Rosenheim: Journal of Physiology, 1907, xxxvi,
p. 211.
17 Patterson and Starling: Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xlvii, p. 143.
18 See Macleod and Pearce: American Journal of Physiology,
1913, xxxii, p. 192. Pavy and Siau: Journal of Physiology, 1903, xxix,
p. 375. Macleod: American Journal of Physiology, 1909, xxiii, p. 278.
19 Locke: Centralblatt für Physiologie, 1900, xiv, p.
671.
20 Schumbcrg: Archiv für Physiologie, 1896, p. 537.
21 Frentzel: Archiv für Physiologie, 1899, Supplement
Band,
p. 145.
22 Lee and Harrold: American Journal of Physiology, 1900, iv,
p. ix.
23 Wilenko: Biochemische Zeitschrift, 1912, xlii, p. 58.
24 Wilenko: Archiv für experimentelle Pathologie und
Pharmakologie, 1913, Ixxi, p. 266.
25 Lusk: Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology
and Medicine, 1914, xi, p. 49. Also Lusk and Riche: Archives of
Internal Medicine, 1914, xiii, p. 68.
28 See Elliott: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xliv, p. 376.
27 Macleod: Diabetes, etc., pp. 64-73.
28 Macleod: Diabetes, etc., pp. 68-72.
29 See Biedl: Die Innere Sekretion, 1913, i. p. 464.
30 Hoskins and Lovellette: Journal of the American Medical
Association, 1914, Ixiii, p. 317.
31 See Haldane and Priestley: Journal of Physiology, 1905,
xxxii, p. 255.
32 Douglas and Haldane: Journal of Physiology, 1909, xxxix,
p. 1.
33 See Januschke and Pollak: Archiv für experimentelle
Pathologie und Pharmakologie, 1911, Ixvi, p. 205. Trendelenburg:
Zentralblatt für Physiologie, 1912, xxvi, p. 1. Jackson: Journal
of
Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 1912, iv, p. 59.
34 Of. Hoskins and McClure: Archives of Internal Medicine,
1912, x, p. 355.
35 Cannon and Hoskins: American Journal of Physiology, 1911,
xxix, p. 275.
36 Anrep: Journal of Physiology, 1912, xlv, p. 307.
37 Gasser and Meek: American Journal of Physiology, 1914,
xxxiv, p. 63.
38 Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1919, 1, p. 399.
39 For evidence and for references to this literature, see
Bang: Der Blutzucker, Wiesbaden, 1913, pp. 104-108.
40 Starkenstein: Loc. cit., p. 94.
41 Macleod: Diabetes, etc., p. 184.
42 Zuntz: Loc. cit., p. 854,
CHAPTER XII
THE ENERGIZING INFLUENCE OF EMOTIONAL EXCITEMENT
The close relation between emotion and muscular action has
long been perceived. As Sherrington * has pointed out, "Emotion 'moves'
us, hence the word itself. If developed in intensity, it impels toward
vigorous movement. Every vigorous movement of the body . . . involves
also the less noticeable cooperation of the viscera, especially of the
circulatory and respiratory. The extra demand made upon the muscles
that move
the frame involves a heightened action of the nutrient organs
which supply to the muscles the material for their energy." The
researches here reported have revealed a number of unsuspected ways in
which muscular action is made more efficient because of emotional
disturbances of the viscera. Every one of the visceral changes that
have been noted the cessation of processes in the alimentary canal
(thus freeing the energy supply for other parts); the shifting of blood
from the abdominal organs, whose activities are deferable,
to the organs immediately essential to muscular exertion (the
lungs, the heart, the central nervous system); the increased vigor of
contraction of the heart; the quick abolition of the effects of
muscular fatigue; the mobilizing of energy-giving sugar in the
circulation - every one of these visceral changes is directly
serviceable in making the organism more effective in the violent
display of energy which fear or rage or pain may involve.
"RESERVOIRS OP POWER"
That the major emotions have an energizing effect has been
commonly recognized.* Darwin testified to having heard, "as a proof of
the exciting nature of anger, that a man when excessively jaded will
sometimes invent imaginary offences and put himself into a passion,
unconsciously for the sake of reinvigorating himself; and," Darwin 2
continues, "since hearing this remark, I have occasionally recognized
its full truth." Under the impulse of fear also, men have been known to
achieve extraordinary feats of running and leaping. McDougall 3 cites
the instance
* Russell (The Pima Indians, United States Bureau of
Ethnology, 1908, p. 243) relates a tale told by the Indians to their
children, in which an injured coyote was chasing some quails. "Finally
the quails got tired," according to the story, "but the coyote did not,
for he was angry and did not feel fatigue."
of an athlete who, when pursued as a boy by a savage animal,
leaped over a wall which he could not again "clear" until he attained
his full stature and strength. The very unusual abilities, both
physical and mental, which men have exhibited in times of stress were
dealt with from the psychological point of view by William James 4 in
one of his last essays. He suggested that in every person there are
"reservoirs of power"
which are not ordinarily called upon, but which are
nevertheless ready to pour forth streams of energy if only the occasion
presents itself. These figurative expressions of the psychologist
receive definite and concrete exemplification, so far as the physical
exhibitions of power are concerned, in the highly serviceable bodily
changes which have been described in the foregoing chapters.
It would doubtless be incorrect to attempt to account for all
the increased strength and tireless endurance, which may be experienced
in periods of great excitement, on the basis of abundant supplies
provided then for muscular contraction, and a special secretion for
avoiding or abolishing the depressive influences of fatigue. Tremors,
muscular twitchings, the assumption of characteristic attitudes, all
indicate that there is an immensely augmented activity of the nervous
system an activity that discharges powerfully even into parts
not directly concerned in struggle, as, for example, into the
muscles of voice, causing peculiar cries or warning notes; into the
muscles of the ears, drawing them back or causing them to stand erect,
and into the small muscles about the lips, tightening them and
revealing the teeth. The typical appearances of human beings, as well
as lower animals, when in the grip of such deeply agitating emotions as
fear and rage, are so well recognized as to constitute a primitive and
common means of judging the nature of the experience
through which the organism is passing. This "pattern"
response of the nervous system to an emotion-
provoking object or situation is probably capable of bringing
into action a much greater number of neurones in the central nervous
system than are likely to be concerned in even a supreme act of
volition. The nervous impulses delivered to the muscles, furthermore,
operate upon organs well supplied with energy-yielding material and
well fortified by rapidly circulating blood and by secreted adrenin,
against quick loss of power because of accumulating waste. Under such
circumstances of excitement the performance of extraordinary
feats of strength or endurance is natural enough.*
* If individual neurones obey the law of either
supreme
action or inaction, the "all-or-none law," the only means of securing a
graded response is through variation of the number of neurones engaged
in action the more, the greater the resulting manifestation of
strength.
In connection with the conception that strong emotion has a
dynamogenic value, it is of interest to note that on occasions when
great demands are likely to be placed on the neuro-muscular system in
the doing of unusual labors, emotional excitement is not uncommonly an
accompaniment. In order to emphasize points in the argument developed
thus far, I propose to cite some examples of the association of
emotional excitement with remarkable exhibitions of power or resistance
to fatigue.
THE EXCITEMENTS AND ENERGIES OP COMPETITIVE SPORTS
Already in an earlier account (see p. 75) I have mentioned
finding sugar in the urine in approximately fifty per cent of a group
of college football players after the most exacting game of the
season's play. As is well understood, such games are heralded far and
wide, loyal supporters of each college may travel hundreds of miles to
attend the contest, enthusiastic meetings of undergraduate students are
held in each college to demonstrate their devotion to the team and
their confidence in its prowess indeed, the arguments for victory,
the songs, the cheering, are likely to be so disturbing to
the players, that before an important contest they are not infrequently
removed from college surroundings in order to avoid being overwrought
when the contest comes.
On the day of the contest the excitement is multiplied
manyfold. There is practically a holiday in college and to a large
extent in the city as well. The streets are filled with eager
supporters of each team as the hosts begin to gather at the field. As
many as 70,000 spectators may be present, each one tense and strongly
partisan. The student bands lead the singing, by thousands of voices,
of songs which urge to the utmost effort for the college; and, in
anticipation, these songs also celebrate the victory.
Into the midst of that huge, cheering, yelling, singing,
flag-waving crowd, the players are welcomed in a special outburst of
these same demonstrations of enthusiasm. Soon the game begins. The
position of every player is known, if not because of previous
acquaintance and recognition, because card-diagrams give the
information. Every important play is seen by the assembled thousands,
and the player who makes it is at once announced to all, and is likely
to be honored by his multitudinous college mates in a special cheer,
ending in his name. Any player who, by infraction of the
rules or failure to do his part, loses ground gained by his team is
also known. The man who is "played out" in efforts to win for his team
and college, and consequently has to leave the field, is welcomed to
the side lines by acclamations suited for a great hero. In short, every
effort is made, through the powerful incentives of censure and a
flaunting recognition, to make each member of the team realize vividly
his responsibility, both personal and as one of a group, for the
supreme, all-important result victory for his college.
This responsibility works tremendously on the emotions of the
players. In the dressing room before a critical contest I have seen a
"gridiron warrior," ready in canvas suit, cleated shoes, and leather
helmet, sitting grimly on a bench, his fists clenched, his jaws tight,
and his face the color of clay. He performed wonderfully when the game
began, and after it was over there was a large percentage of sugar in
his urine ! Probably no sport requires a more sustained and extreme
display of neuro-muscular effort than American football. And from the
foregoing description of the conditions that surround the contests it
is easy to realize
that they conspire to arouse in the players excitements which
would bring forth very efficiently the bodily reserves for use in the
fierce struggle which the game requires.
What is true of football is true, though perhaps to a less
degree, of the racing sports, as running and rowing. Again great
multitudes attend the events, the contests are followed closely from
beginning to end, and as the goal is approached the cheering and cries
for victory gather in volume and intensity as if arranged for a
thrilling climax. The whole setting is most highly favorable to the
dramatic development of an acme of excitement as the moment approaches
when the last desperate effort to win is put forth.
FRENZY AND ENDURANCE IN CEREMONIAL AND OTHER DANCES
Dancing, which formed a significant feature of primitive
rituals, has always been accompanied by exciting conditions, and not
unusually was an exhibition of remarkable endurance. In the transfer of
the Ark to Zion there were processions and sacrifices, and King David
"danced before the Lord with all his might." Mooney 5 in his account of
dances among the American Indians tells of a young man who in one of
the ceremonials
danced three days and nights without food, drink or sleep. In
such a terrible ordeal the favoring presence of others, who through
group action help to stimulate both the excitement and the activities,
must be an important element in prolonging the efforts of the
individual.
In the history of religious manias 6 there are many instances
of large numbers of people becoming frenzied and then showing
extraordinary endurance while dancing. In 1374 a mania broke forth in
Germany, the Netherlands and France, in which the victims claimed to
dance in honor of Saint John. Men and women went about dancing hand in
hand, in pairs, or in a circle, on the streets, in the churches, at
their homes, or wherever they might be, hour after hour without rest.
While dancing they sang, uttered cries, and saw visions. Whole
companies of .these crazy fanatics went dancing along the public roads
and into the cities,
until they had to be interfered with.
In 1740 an extraordinary sect, known as the "Jumpers," arose
in Wales. According to the description given by Wesley, their exercises
were not unlike those of certain frenzied states among the Indians.
"After the preaching was over," Wesley 7 wrote, "anyone who pleased
gave out a verse of a hymn; and this they sung over and over again,
with all their might and main, thirty or forty times, till some of them
worked themselves into a sort of drunkenness or madness; they were then
violently agitated, and leaped up and
down in all manner of postures, frequently for hours
together." There were sometimes thousands at a single meeting of the
Jumpers, shouting out their excitement and ready to leap for joy. 8
Wesley has also described instances of tremendous emotional
outburst at Methodist meetings which he addressed. "Some were torn with
a kind of convulsive motion in every part of their bodies, and that so
violently that often four or five persons could not hold one of them. I
have seen many hysterical or epileptic fits," he wrote, "but none of
them were like these in many respects."
Among the dervishes likewise the dance is accompanied by
intense excitement and apparently tireless movements. "The cries of
'Ya
Allah !' are increased doubly, as also those of 'Ya Hoo !' with
frightful howlings shrieked by the dervishes together in the dance." .
. . "There was no regularity in their dancing, but each seemed to be
performing the antics of a madman; now moving his body up and down; the
next moment turning round, then using odd gesticulations with his arms,
next jumping, and sometimes screaming." . . .
"At the moment when they would seem to stop from sheer
exhaustion the sheikh makes a point of exciting them to new efforts by
walking through their midst, making also himself most violent
movements. He is next replaced by two elders, who double the quickness
of the step and the agitation of the body; they even straighten
themselves up from time to time, and excite the envy or emulation of
others in their astonishing efforts to continue the dance until their
strength is entirely exhausted." Such is the frenzy thus developed
that the performers may be subjected to severe pain, yet only
show signs of elation.
In all these dances the two most marked features are the
intense excitement of those who engage in them and the very remarkable
physical endurance which they manifest. Although there is no direct
evidence, such as was obtained in examining the football players, that
bodily changes favorable to great neuro-muscular exertion are developed
in these furies of fanaticism, it is highly probable that they are so
developed, and that the feats of fortitude which are performed are to a
large extent explicable on the basis of a "tapping of the
reservoirs of power" through the emotional excitement.
THE FIERCE EMOTIONS AND STRUGGLES OF BATTLE
Throughout the discussion of the probable significance of the
bodily changes in pain and great emotion, the value of these changes in
the struggles of conflict or escape was emphasized. In human beings as
well as in lower animals the wildest passions are aroused when the
necessities of combat become urgent. One needs only to glance at the
history of warfare to observe that when the primitive emotions of anger
and hatred are permitted full sway, men who have been considerate and
thoughtful of their fellows and their fellows'
rights suddenly may turn into infuriated savages,
slaughtering innocent women and children, mutilating
the wounded, burning, ravaging, and looting, with all
the wild fervor of demons. It is in such excesses of emotional
turbulence that the most astonishing instances of prolonged exertion
and incredible endurance are to be found.
Probably the fiercest struggles between men that are recorded
are those which occurred when the wager of battle was a means of
determining innocence or guilt. In the corners of the plot selected for
the combat a bier was prepared for each participant, as a symbol that
the struggle was for life or death. Each was attended by his relatives
and followers, and by his father confessor.10 After each had prayed to
God for help in the coming combat, the weapons were selected, the
sacrament was administered, and the battle was begun.
The principals fought to the end with continuous and brutal
ferocity, resembling the desperate encounters of wild beasts. A fairly
illustrative example is furnished in an incident which followed the
assassination of Charles the Good of Flanders in 1127. One of the
accomplices, a knight named Guy, was challenged for complicity by
another named Herman. Both were renowned warriors. Herman was speedily
unhorsed by Guy, who with his lance frustrated all Herman's attempts to
remount. Then Herman disabled Guy's horse, and the combat was renewed
on foot with swords. Equally skilful in fence, they continued the
struggle
till fatigue compelled them to drop sword and shield,
whereupon they wrestled for the mastery. Guy threw his antagonist, fell
on him, and beat him in the face with his gauntlets till he seemed to
be motionless; but Herman had quietly slipped his hand below the
other's coat of mail and, grasping the testicles, with a mighty effort
wrenched them away. Immediately Guy fell over and expired. 11
In such terrific fights as these, conducted in the extremes
of rage and hate, the mechanisms for reenforcing the parts of the body
which are of primary importance in the struggle are brought fully into
action and are of utmost value in securing victory.
THE STIMULATING INFLUENCE OF WITNESSES AND OF MUSIC
It is noteworthy that in all the instances thus far cited in
the great games, in dancing, and in fighting two factors are present
that are well known to have an augmenting effect both in the full
development of emotions and in the performance of unusual muscular
labors. One of these is the crowd of witnesses or participants, who
contribute the "mob spirit" that tends to carry the actions of the
individual far beyond the limits set by any personal considerations or
prudencies. The other is the influence of music. As Darwin
long ago indicated, music has a wonderful power of recalling
in a vague and indefinite manner strong emotions which have been felt
by our ancestors in long-past ages. Especially is this true of martial
music. For the grim purposes of war the reed and the lute are
grotesquely ill-suited; to rouse men to action strident brass and the
jarring instruments of percussion are used in full force. The influence
of martial music on some persons is so profound as to cause the muscles
to tremble and tears to come to the eyes both indications of
the deep stirring of emotional responses in the body. And
when deeds of fortitude and fierce exertion are to be performed the
effectiveness of such music in rousing the aggressive emotions has long
been recognized. The Romans charged their foes amid the blasts of
trumpets and horns. The ancient Germans rushed to battle, their forces
spurred by the sounds of drums, flutes, cymbals and clarions. There is
a tradition that the Hungarian troops are the worst in Europe, until
their bands begin to play then they are the best! The
late General Linevitch is quoted as saying: "Music is one of
the most vital ammunitions of the Russian army. Without music a Russian
soldier would be dull, cowardly, brutal and inefficient. From music he
absorbs a magic power of endurance, and forgets the sufferings and
mortality. It is a divine dynamite." And Napoleon is said to have
testified that the weird and barbaric tunes of the Cossack regiments
infuriated them to such rage that they wiped out the cream of his army.
12 A careful consideration of the use of martial music in warfare would
perhaps bring further interesting evidence that its function is to
reinforce the bodily
changes that attend the belligerent emotions.
Only a few instances of the combination of extreme pain,
rage, terror or excitement, and tremendous muscular power have been
given in the preceding pages. Doubtless in numerous other conditions
these two groups of phenomena occur together. In the lives of firemen
and the police, in the experiences of escaping prisoners, of
shipwrecked sailors, in the struggles between pioneers and their savage
enemies, in accounts of forced marches or retreats, search would reveal
many examples of such bodily disturbances as have been
described in earlier chapters as augmenting the effectiveness
of muscular efforts, and such exhibitions of power or endurance as are
evidently far beyond the ordinary. There is every reason for believing
that, were the conditions favorable to experimental testing, it would
be possible to demonstrate and perhaps to measure the addition to the
dynamics of bodily action that appears as the accompaniment of violent
emotional disturbance.
THE FEELING OF POWER
In this connection it is highly significant that in times of
strong excitement there is not infrequent testimony to a sense of
overwhelming power that sweeps in like a sudden tide and lifts the
person to a new high level of ability. A friend of mine, whose nature
is somewhat choleric, has told me that when he is seized with anger, he
is also possessed by an intense conviction that he could crush and
utterly destroy the object of his hostility. And I have heard a
football player confess that just before the final game such an access
of strength
seemed to come to him that he felt able, on the signal, to
crouch and with a jump go crashing through any ordinary door. There is
intense satisfaction in these moments of supreme elation, when the body
is at its acme of accomplishment.
And it is altogether probable that the critical dangers of
adventure have a fascination because fear is thrilling, and extrication
from a predicament, by calling forth all the bodily resources and
setting them to meet the challenge of the difficulty, yields many of
the joys of conquest. For these reasons vigorous men go forth to seek
dangers and to run large chances of serious injury. "Danger makes us
more alive. We so love to strive that we come to love the fear that
gives us strength for conflict. Fear is not only something to be
escaped from to a place or state of safety, but welcomed as an arsenal
of augmented strength." 13 And thus in
the hazardous sports, in mountain climbing, in the hunting of
big game, and in the tremendous adventure of war, risks and excitement
and the sense of power surge up together, setting free unsuspected
energies, and bringing vividly to consciousness memorable fresh
revelations of the possibilities of achievement.
REFERENCES
1 Sherrington: The Integrative Action of the Nervous System,
New York, 1906, p. 265.
2 Darwin: The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals, New
York, 1905, p. 79.
3 McDougall: Introduction to Social Psychology, London, 1908,
p. 50.
4 James: The Energies of Men, p. 227, in Memories and
Studies, New York, 1911.
5 Mooney: The Ghost-Dance Religion, United States Bureau of
Ethnology, 1892-3, p. 924.
6 Schaff: Religious Encyclopedia, New York, 1908, iii, p.
346.
7 Southey: Life of Charles Wesley, New York, 1820, ii, p.
164.
8 Southey: Loc. cit., i, p. 240.
9 Brown: The Dervishes, London, 1868, pp. 218-222, 260.
10 Majer: Geschichte der Ordalien, Jena, 1796, pp. 258- 261.
11 Lea: Superstition and Force, Philadelphia, 1892, p. 178.
12 Narodny: Musical America, 1914, xx, No. 14.
13 Hall: American Journal of Psychology, 1914, xxv, p.
CHAPTER XIII
THE NATURE OF HUNGER
On the same plane with pain and the dominant emotions of fear
and anger, as agencies which determine the action of organisms, is the
sensation of hunger. It is a sensation so peremptory, so disagreeable,
so tormenting, that men have committed crimes in order to assuage it.
It has led to cannibalism, even among the civilized. It has resulted in
suicide. And it has defeated armies for the aggressive spirit becomes
detached from larger loyalties and turns personal and selfish as hunger
pangs increase in vigor and insistence.
In 1905, while observing in myself the rhythmic sounds
produced by the activities of the alimentary tract, I had occasion to
note that the sensation of hunger was not constant but recurrent, and
that the moment of its disappearance was often associated with a rather
loud gurgling sound as heard through the stethoscope. This and other
evidence, indicative of a source of the hunger sensations in the
contractions of the digestive canal, I reported in 1911.1 That same
year, with the help of one of my students, A. L. Washburn, I obtained
final
proof for this inference.
APPETITE AND HUNGER
The sensations of appetite and hunger are so complex and so
intimately interrelated that any discussion of either sensation is sure
to go astray unless at the start there is clear understanding of the
meanings of the terms. The view has been propounded that appetite is
the first degree of hunger, the mild and pleasant stage, agreeable in
character; and that hunger itself is a more advanced condition,
disagreeable and even painful
the unpleasant result of not satisfying the appetite. 2 On
this basis appetite and hunger would differ only quantitatively.
Another view, which seems more justifiable, is that the two experiences
are fundamentally different.
Careful observation indicates that appetite is related to
previous sensations of taste and smell of food. Delightful or
disgusting tastes and odors, associated with this or that edible
substance, determine the appetite. It has, therefore, important psychic
elements in its composition. Thus, by taking thought, we can anticipate
the odor of a delicious beefsteak or the taste of peaches and cream,
and in that imagination we can find pleasure. In the realization,
direct effects in the senses of taste and smell give still further
delight. As already noted in the first chapter, observations on
experimental animals and on human beings have shown
that the pleasures of both anticipation and realization, by
stimulating the flow of saliva and gastric juice, play a highly
significant role in the initiation of digestive processes.
Among prosperous people, supplied with abundance of food, the
appetite seems sufficient to ensure for bodily needs a proper supply of
nutriment. We eat because dinner is announced, because by eating we
avoid unpleasant consequences, and because food is placed before us in
delectable form and with tempting tastes and odors. Under less easy
circumstances, however, the body needs are supplied through the much
stronger and more insistent demands of hunger.
The sensation of hunger is difficult to describe, but almost
everyone from childhood has felt at times that dull ache or gnawing
pain referred to the lower mid-chest region and the epigastrium, which
may take imperious control of human actions. As Sternberg has pointed
out, hunger may be sufficiently insistent to force the taking of food
which is so distasteful that it not only fails to rouse appetite, but
may even produce nausea. The hungry being gulps his food with a rush.
The pleasures of appetite are not for him he wants
quantity rather than quality, and he wants it at once.
Hunger and appetite are, therefore, widely different in
physiological basis, in localization and in psychic elements. Hunger
may be satisfied while the appetite still calls. Who is still hungry
when the tempting dessert is served, and yet are there any who refuse
it, on the plea that they no longer need it? On the other hand,
appetite may be in abeyance while hunger is goading.,3 What ravenous
boy is critical of his food? Do we not all know that "hunger is the
best sauce"? Although the two sensations may thus exist separately,
they
nevertheless have the same function of leading to the intake
of food, and they usually appear together. Indeed, the cooperation of
hunger and appetite is probably the reason for their being so
frequently confused.
THE SENSATION OP HUNGER
Hunger may be described as having a central core and certain
more or less variable accessories. The peculiar dull ache of
hungriness, referred to the epigastrium, is usually the organism's
first strong demand for food; and when the initial order is not obeyed,
the sensation is likely to grow into a highly uncomfortable pang or
gnawing, less definitely localized as it becomes more intense. This may
be regarded as the essential feature of hunger. Besides the dull ache,
however, lassitude and drowsiness may appear, or faintness, or violent
headache, or irritability and restlessness such that
continuous effort in ordinary affairs becomes increasingly difficult.
That these states differ much with individuals headache in one and
faintness in another, for example indicates that they do not constitute
the central fact of hunger, but are more or less inconstant
accompaniments. The "feeling of emptiness," which has been mentioned as
an important element of the experience, 4 is an inference rather than a
distinct datum of consciousness, and can likewise be eliminated from
further consideration. The dull pressing sensation is left, therefore,
as the constant characteristic,
the central fact, to be examined in detail.
Hunger can evidently be regarded from the psychological point
of view, and discussed solely on the basis of introspection; or it can
be studied with reference to its antecedents and to the physiological
conditions which accompany it a consideration which requires the use of
both objective methods and subjective observation. This
psychophysiological treatment of the subject will be deferred till the
last. Certain theories which have been advanced with regard to hunger,
and which have been given more or less credit, must first be examined.
Two main theories have been advocated. The first is supported
by contentions that hunger is a general sensation, arising at no
special region of the body, but having a local reference. This theory
has been more widely credited by physiologists and psychologists than
the other. The other is supported by evidence that hunger has a local
source and therefore a local reference. In the course of our
examination of these views we shall have opportunity to consider some
pertinent new observations.
THE THEORY THAT HUNGER IS A GENERAL SENSATION
The conception that hunger arises from a general condition of
the body rests in turn on the notion that, as the body uses up
material, the blood becomes impoverished. Schiff 5 advocated this
notion, and suggested that poverty of the blood in food substance
affects the tissues in such manner that they demand a new supply. The
nerve cells of the brain share in this general shortage of provisions,
and because of internal changes, give rise to the sensation. Thus is
hunger explained as an experience dependent on the body as a whole.
Three classes of evidence are cited in support of this view:
1. "Hunger increases as time passes" a partial statement. The
development of hunger as time passes is a common observation which
quite accords with the assumption that the condition of the body and
the state of the blood are becoming constantly worse, so long as the
need, once established, is not satisfied.
While it is true that with the lapse of time hunger increases
as the supply of body nutriment decreases, this concomitance is not
proof that the sensation arises directly from a serious encroachment on
the store of food materials. If this argument were valid we should
expect hunger to become more and more distressing until death follows
from starvation. There is abundant evidence that the sensation is not
thus intensified; on the contrary, during continued fasting hunger, at
least in some persons, wholly disappears after the first few
days. Luciani, 6 who carefully recorded the experience of the
faster Succi, states that after a certain time the hunger feelings
vanish and do not return. And he tells of two dogs that showed no signs
of hunger after the third or fourth day of fasting; thereafter they
remained quite passive in the presence of food. Tigerstedt,7 who also
has studied the metabolism of starvation, declares that although the
desire to eat is very great during the
first day of the ordeal, the unpleasant sensations disappear
early, and that at the end of the fast the subject may have to force
himself to take nourishment. The subject, 'J, A.,' studied by
Tigerstedt and his co-workers, 8 reported that after the fourth day of
fasting, he had no disagreeable feelings.
Carrington, 9 after examining many persons who, to better
their health, abstained from eating for different periods, records that
"habit-hunger" usually lasts only two or three days and, if plenty of
water is drunk, does not last longer than three days. Viterbi, 10 a
Corsican lawyer condemned to death for political causes, determined to
escape execution by depriving his body of food and drink. During the
eighteen days that he lived he kept careful notes. On the third day the
sensation of hunger departed, and although thereafter thirst
came and went, hunger never returned. Still further evidence
of the same character could be cited, but enough has already been given
to show that after the first few days of fasting the hunger feelings
may wholly cease. On the theory that hunger is a manifestation of
bodily need, are we to suppose that, in the course of starvation, the
body is mysteriously not in need after the third day, and that
therefore the sensation of hunger disappears ? The absurdity of such a
view is obvious.
2. "Hunger may be felt though the stomach be full" a selected
alternative. Instances of duodenal fistula in man have been carefully
studied, which have shown that a modified sensation of hunger may be
felt when the stomach is full. A famous case described by Busch u has
been repeatedly used as evidence. His patient, who lost nutriment
through a duodenal fistula, was hungry soon after eating, and felt
satisfied only when the
chyme was restored to the intestine through the distal
fistulous opening. As food is absorbed mainly through the intestinal
wall, the inference is direct that the general bodily state, and not
the local conditions of the alimentary canal, must account for the
patient's feelings.
A full consideration of the evidence from cases of duodenal
fistula cannot so effectively be presented now as later. That in
Busch's case hunger disappeared while food was being taken is, as we
shall see, quite significant. It may be that the restoration of chyme
to the intestine quieted hunger, not because nutriment was thus
introduced into the body, but because the presence of material altered
the nature of gastro-intestinal
activity. The basis for this suggestion will be given in due
course.
3. "Animals may eat eagerly after section of their vagus and
splanchnic nerves" a fallacious argument. The third support for the
view that hunger has a general origin in the body is derived from
observations on experimental animals. By severance of the vagus and
splanchnic nerves, the lower esophagus, the stomach and the small
intestine can be wholly separated from the central nervous system.
Animals thus operated upon nevertheless eat food placed before them,
and may indeed manifest some eagerness for it. 12 How is this behavior
to be accounted for when the possibility of local stimulation has been
eliminated save by assuming a central origin of the impulse to eat?
The fallacy of this evidence, though repeatedly overlooked,
is easily shown. We have already seen that appetite as well as hunger
may lead to the taking of food. Indeed, the animal with all
gastro-intestinal nerves cut may have the same incentive to eat that a
well-fed man may have, who delights in the pleasurable taste and smell
of food and knows nothing of hunger pangs. Even when the nerves of
taste are cut, as they were in
Longet's experiments, 13 sensations of smell are still
possible, as well as agreeable associations which can be roused by
sight. More than fifty years ago Ludwig 14 pointed out that, even if
all the nerves were severed, psychic reasons could be given for the
taking of food, and yet because animals eat after one or another set of
nerves is eliminated, the conclusion has been drawn by various writers
that the nerves in question are thereby proved to be not concerned in
the sensation of hunger. Evidently, since hunger is not required for
eating, the act of eating is no testimony whatever that the animal is
hungry, and, after the nerves have been severed, is no proof that
hunger is of central origin.
WEAKNESS OF THE ASSUMPTIONS UNDERLYING THE THEORY THAT HUNGER
IS A GENERAL SENSATION
The evidence thus far examined has been shown to afford only
shaky support for the theory that hunger is a general sensation. The
theory, furthermore, is weak in its fundamental assumptions. There is
no clear indication, for example, that the blood undergoes or has
undergone any marked change, chemical or physical, when the first
stages of hunger appear. There is no evidence of any direct chemical
stimulation .of the gray matter of the cerebral cortex. Indeed,
attempts to excite the gray matter artificially by chemical agents have
been without, results; 15 and even electrical stimulation, which is
effective, must, in order to produce movements, be so powerful that the
movements have been attributed to excitation of underlying white matter
rather than cells in the gray. This insensitivity of cortical cells to
direct stimulation is not at all favorable to the notion that they are
sentinels set to warn against too great diminution of bodily supplies.
BODY NEED MAY EXIST WITHOUT HUNGER
Still further evidence opposed to the theory that hunger
results directly from the using up of organic stores is found in
patients suffering from fever. Metabolism in fever patients is
augmented, body substance is destroyed to such a degree that the weight
of the patient may be greatly reduced, and yet the sensation of hunger
under these conditions of increased need is wholly lacking.
Again, if a person is hungry and takes food, the sensation is
suppressed soon afterwards, long before any considerable amount of
nutriment could be digested and absorbed, and therefore long before the
blood and the general bodily condition, if previously altered, could be
restored to normal.
Furthermore, persons exposed to privation have testified that
hunger can be temporarily suppressed by swallowing indigestible
materials. Certainly scraps of leather and bits of moss, not to mention
clay eaten by the Otomacs, would not materially compensate for large
organic losses. In rebuttal to this argument the comment has been made
that central states as a rule can be readily overwhelmed by peripheral
stimulation, and just as sleep, for example, can be abolished by
bathing the temples, so hunger can be abolished by irritating the
gastric walls. 16 This comment is beside the point, for it meets the
issue by merely assuming
as true the condition under discussion. The absence of hunger
during the ravages of fever, and its quick abolition after food or even
indigestible stuff is swallowed, still further weakens the argument,
therefore, that the sensation arises directly from lack of nutriment in
the body.
THE THEORY THAT HUNGER IS OF GENERAL ORIGIN DOES NOR EXPLAIN
THE QUICK ONSET AND THE PERIODICITY OF THE SENSATION
Many persons have noted that hunger has a sharp onset. A
person may be tramping in the woods or working in the fields, where
fixed attention is not demanded, and without premonition may feel the
abrupt arrival of the characteristic ache. The expression "grub-struck"
is a picturesque description of this experience. If this
sudden arrival of the sensation corresponds to the general
bodily state, the change in the general bodily state must occur with
like suddenness or have a critical point at which the sensation is
instantly precipitated. There is no evidence whatever that either of
these conditions occurs in the course of metabolism.
Another peculiarity of hunger, which I have already
mentioned, is its intermittency. It may come and go several times in
the course of a few hours. Furthermore, while the sensation is
prevailing, its intensity is not uniform, but marked by ups and downs.
In some instances the ups and downs change to a periodic presence and
absence without change of rate. In my own experience the hunger pangs
came and went on one occasion as follows:
and so on, for ten minutes longer. Again in this relation,
the intermittent and periodic character of hunger would require, on the
theory under examination, that the bodily supplies be intermittently
and periodically insufficient. During one moment the absence of hunger
would imply an abundance of nutriment in the organism, ten seconds
later the presence of hunger would imply that the stores had been
suddenly reduced, ten seconds later still the absence of hunger would
imply a sudden renewal of plenty. Such zig-zag shifts of the general
bodily state may not be impossible, but from all that is known of the
course of metabolism, such quick changes are highly improbable. The
periodicity of hunger, therefore, is further evidence against the
theory that the sensation has a general basis in the body.
THE THEORY THAT HUNGER IS OF GENERAL ORIGIN DOES NOT EXPLAIN
THE LOCAL REFERENCE
The last objection to this theory is that it does not account
for the most common feature of hunger namely, the reference of the
sensation to the region of the stomach. Schiff and others 17 who have
supported the theory have met this objection by two contentions. First
they have pointed out that the sensation is not always referred to the
stomach. Schiff interrogated ignorant soldiers regarding the local
reference; several
indicated the neck or chest, twenty-three the sternum, four
were uncertain of any region, and two only designated the stomach. In
other words, the stomach region was most rarely mentioned.
The second contention against the importance of local
reference is that such evidence is fallacious. An armless man may feel
tinglings which seem to arise in fingers which have long since ceased
to be a portion of his body. The fact that he experiences such
tinglings and ascribes them to dissevered parts, does not prove that
the sensation originates in those parts. And similarly the assignment
of the ache of hunger to any special region of the body does not
demonstrate that the ache arises from that region. Such are the
arguments against a local origin of hunger.
Concerning these arguments we may recall, first, Schiff's
admission that the soldiers he questioned were too few to give
conclusive evidence. Further, the testimony of most of them that hunger
seemed to originate in the chest or region of the sternum cannot be
claimed as unfavorable to a peripheral source of the sensation. The
description of feelings which develop from disturbances within the body
is almost always indefinite. As Head 18 and others have shown,
conditions in a viscus which give rise to sensation are likely not to
be attributed to the viscus, but to related skin areas. Under such
circumstances we do not dismiss the testimony as worthless merely
because it may not point precisely to the source of the trouble. On the
contrary, we use such testimony constantly as a basis for judging
internal disorders. With regard to the contention that reference to the
periphery is not proof of the peripheral origin of a sensation, we may
answer that the force of that contention depends on the amount of
accessory evidence which is available. Thus if we see an object come
into contact with a finger, we are justified in assuming that the
simultaneous sensation of touch which we refer to that finger has
resulted from the contact, and is not a purely central experience
accidentally attributed to an outlying member. Similarly in the case of
hunger all that we need as support for the peripheral reference of the
sensation is proof that conditions occur there, simultaneously with
hunger pangs, which might reasonably be regarded as giving rise to
those pangs.
With the requirement in mind that peripheral conditions be
adequate, let us examine the state of the fasting stomach to see
whether, indeed, conditions may be present in times of hunger which
would sustain the theory that hunger has a local outlying source.
HUNGER NOT DUE TO EMPTINESS OF THE STOMACH
Among the suggestions which have been offered to account for
a peripheral origin of the sensation is that of attributing it to
emptiness of the stomach. By use of the stomach tube Nicolai 19 found
that when his subjects had their first intimation of hunger the stomach
was quite empty. But, in other instances, after lavage of the stomach,
the sensation did not appear for intervals varying between one and a
half and three and a half
hours. During these intervals the stomach must have been
empty, and yet no sensation was experienced. The same testimony was
given long before by Beaumont, 20 who, from his observations on Alexis
St. Martin, declared that hunger arises some time after the stomach is
normally evacuated. Mere emptiness of the organ, therefore, does not
explain the phenomenon.
HUNGER NOT DUE TO HYDROCHLORIC ACID IN THE EMPTY STOMACH
A second theory, apparently suggested by observations on
cases of hyperacidity, is that the ache or pang is due to the natural
hydrochloric acid of the stomach but secreted while the organ is empty.
Again the facts are hostile. Nicolai 21 reported that the gastric
wash-water from his hungry subjects was neutral or only slightly acid.
This testimony confirms Beaumont's statement, and is in complete
agreement with the results of
gastric examination of fasting animals reported by numerous
experimenters. There is no secretion into the empty stomach during the
first days of starvation. Furthermore, persons suffering from absence
of hydrochloric acid (achylia gastrica) declare that they have normal
feelings of hunger. Hydrochloric acid cannot, therefore, be called upon
to account for the sensation.
HUNGER NOT DUE TO TURGESCENCE OF THE GASTRIC MUCOUS MEMBRANE
Another theory, which was first advanced by Beaumont, 22 is
that hunger arises from turgescence of the gastric glands. The
disappearance of the pangs as fasting continues has been accounted for
by supposing that the gastric glands share in the general depletion of
the body, and that thus the turgescenee is relieved.* This turgescence
theory has commended itself to several recent writers. Thus Luciani 23
has accepted it, and by adding the idea that nerves distributed to the
mucosa are
* A bettor explanation perhaps is afforded by
Boldireff's
discovery that at the end of two or three days the stomachs of fasting
dogs begin to secrete gastric juice and continue the secretion
indefinitely. (Boldireff, Archives Biologiques de St. Petersburg, 1905,
xi, p. 98.)
specially sensitive to deprivation of food lie accounts for
the hunger pangs. Also Valenti 24 declared a few years ago that
the turgescence theory of Beaumont is the only one with a semblance of
truth in it. The experimental work reported by these two investigators,
however, does not necessarily sustain the turgescence theory. Luciani
severed the previously exposed vagi after cocainizing them, and Valenti
merely cocainized the nerves; the fasting dogs, eager to eat a few
minutes previous to this operation, now ran about
as before, but when offered food, licked and smelled it, but
did not take it. This total neglect of the food lasted varying periods
up to two hours. The vagus nerves seem, indeed, to convey impulses
which affect the procedure of eating, but there is no clear evidence
that those impulses arise from distention of the gland cells. The
turgescence theory, moreover, does not explain the effect of taking
indigestible material into the stomach. According to Pawlow, and to
others who have observed human beings, the chewing and swallowing of
unappetizing stuff does not cause any secretion of gastric juice (see
p. 8). Yet such stuff when
swallowed will cause the disappearance of hunger, and Nicolai
found that the sensation could be abolished
by simply introducing a stomach sound. It is highly
improbable that the turgescence of the gastric glands can be reduced by
either of these procedures. The turgescence theory, furthermore, does
not explain the quick onset of hunger, or its intermittent and periodic
character. That the cells are repeatedly swollen and contracted within
periods a few seconds in duration is almost inconceivable. For these
reasons, therefore, the theory that hunger results from turgescence of
the gastric mucosa can reasonably be rejected.
HUNGER THE RESULT OF CONTRACTIONS
There remain to be considered, as a possible cause of
hunger-pangs, contractions of the stomach and other parts of the
alimentary canal. This suggestion is not new. Sixty-nine years ago
Weber 25 declared his belief that "strong contraction of the muscle
fibres of the wholly empty stomach, whereby its cavity disappears,
makes a part of the sensation which we call hunger." Vierordt 26 drew
the same inference twenty-five years later (in 1871), and since then
Ewald, Knapp, and Hertz have declared their adherence to this view.
These
writers have not brought forward any direct evidence for
their conclusion, though Hertz has cited Boldireff's observations
on
fasting dogs as probably accounting for what he terms "the gastric
constituent of the sensation."
THE EMPTY STOMACH AND INTESTINE CONTRACT
The argument commonly used against the gastric contraction
theory is that the stomach is not
energetically active when empty. Thus Schiff 27 stated, "The
movements of the empty stomach are rare and much less energetic than
during digestion." Luciani 28 expressed his disbelief by asserting that
gastric movements are much more active during gastric digestion than at
other times, and cease almost entirely when the stomach has discharged
its contents. And Valenti 29 stated (1910), "We know very well that
gastric movements are exaggerated while digestion is proceeding in the
stomach, but when the organ is empty
they are more rare and much less pronounced," and, therefore,
they cannot account for hunger.
Evidence opposed to these suppositions has been in existence
for many years. In 1899 Bettmann 30 called attention to the contracted
condition of the stomach after several days' fast. In 1902 Wolff 31
reported that after forty-eight hours without food the stomach of the
cat may be so small as to look like a slightly enlarged duodenum. In a
similar circumstance I have noticed the same extraordinary smallness of
the organ, especially in the pyloric half. The anatomist His 32 also
recorded his observation of the phenomenon. In 1905
Boldireff 33 demonstrated that the whole gastrointestinal
tract has a periodic activity while not digesting.
Each period of activity lasts from twenty to thirty minutes,
and is characterized in the stomach by rhythmic contractions ten to
twenty in number. These contractions, Boldireff reports, may be
stronger than during digestion, and his published records clearly
support this statement. The intervals of repose between periodic
recurrences of the contractions lasted from one and a half to two and a
half hours. Especially noteworthy is Boldireff's observation that
if
fasting is continued for two or three days, the groups of contractions
appear at
gradually longer intervals and last for gradually shorter
periods, and thereupon, as the gastric glands begin continuous
secretion, all movements cease.
OBSERVATIONS SUGGESTING THAT CONTRACTIONS CAUSE HUNGER
The research, previously mentioned, on the rhythmic sounds
produced by the digestive process, I was engaged in when Boldireff's
paper was published. That contractions of the alimentary canal on a
gaseous content might explain the hunger pangs which I had noticed
seemed probable at that time, especially in the light of Boldireff's
observations. Indeed, Boldireff 34 himself had considered hunger in
relation to the activities he described, but solely with the idea that
hunger might provoke them; and since the activities dwindled in force
and frequency as time passed, whereas, in his belief, they should have
become more pronounced, he abandoned the notion of any relation between
the phenomena. Did not Boldireff misinterpret
his own observations? When he was considering whether hunger
might cause the contractions, did he not overlook the possibility that
the contractions might cause hunger? A number of experiences have led
to the conviction that Boldireff did, indeed, fail to perceive part of
the significance of his results. For example, I have noticed the
disappearance of a hunger pang as gas was heard gurgling upward through
the cardia. That
the gas was rising rather than being forced downward was
proved by its regurgitation immediately after the sound was heard. In
all probability the pressure that forced the gas from the stomach was
the cause of the preceding sensation of hunger.
Again the sensation can be momentarily abolished a few
seconds after swallowing a small accumulation of saliva or a
teaspoonful of water. If the stomach is in strong contraction in
hunger, this result can be accounted for, in accordance with the
observations of Lieb and myself, 35 as due to the inhibition of the
contraction by swallowing. Thus also could be explained the prompt
vanishing of the ache soon after we begin to eat, for repeated
swallowing results in continued inhibition.* Furthermore, Ducceschi's
discovery
36 that hydrochloric
* The absence of hunger in Busch's patient while food
was
being eaten (see p. 239) can also be accounted for in this manner.
acid diminishes the tonus of the pyloric portion of the
stomach may have its application here; the acid would be secreted as
food is taken and would then cause relaxation of the very region which
is most strongly contracted.
THE CONCOMITANCE OF CONTRACTIONS AND HUNGER IN MAN
Although the evidence above outlined had led me to the
conviction that hunger results from contractions of the alimentary
canal, direct proof was still lacking. In order to learn whether such
proof might be secured, Washburn determined to become accustomed to the
presence of a rubber tube in the esophagus.* Almost every day for
several weeks Washburn introduced as far as the stomach a small tube,
to the lower end of which was attached a soft-rubber balloon about 8
centimeters in diameter. The tube was thus carried about each time for
two or three hours. After this preliminary experience the introduction
of the tube and its
presence in the gullet and stomach were not at all
disturbing. When a record was to be taken, the balloon, placed just
within the stomach, was moderately distended with air, and was
connected with a water manometer ending in a cylindrical chamber 3.5
centimeters wide. A float recorder resting on
* Nicolai (loc. cit.) reported that although the
introduction
of a stomach tube at first abolished hunger in his subjects, with
repeated use the effects became insignificant.
the water in the chamber permitted registering any
contractions of the fundus of the stomach. On the days of observation
Washburn would abstain from breakfast, or eat sparingly; and without
taking any luncheon would appear in the laboratory about two o'clock.
The recording apparatus was arranged as above described. In order to
avoid any error that might arise from artificial pressure on the
balloon, a pneumograph, fastened below the ribs, was made to record the
movements of the abdominal wall. Uniformity of these movements
would show that no special contractions of the abdominal
muscles were made. Between the records of gastric pressure and
abdominal movement, time was marked in minutes, and an electromagnetic
signal traced a line which could be altered by pressing a key. All
these recording arrangements were out of Washburn's sight; he sat with
one hand at the key, ready whenever the sensation of hunger was
experienced to make the current which moved the signal.
Sometimes the observations were started before any hunger was
noted; at other times the sensation, after running a course, gave way
to a feeling of fatigue. Under either of these circumstances there were
no contractions of the stomach. When Washburn stated that he was
hungry, however, powerful contractions of the stomach were invariably
being registered. As in my own earlier experience, the sensations were
characterized by periodic recurrences with free intervals, or by
periodic accesses of an uninterrupted ache. The record of Washburn's
introspection of his hunger pangs agreed closely with the record of his
gastric contractions
Almost invariably, however, the contraction nearly reached
its maximum before the record of the sensation was started (see Fig.
37). This fact may be regarded as evidence that the contraction
precedes the sensation, and not rice versa, as Boldireff considered it.
The contractions were about a half-minute in duration and the intervals
between varied from thirty to ninety seconds, with an average of about
one minute. The
augmentations of intragastric pressure in Washburn ranged
between eleven and thirteen in twenty minutes; I had previously counted
in myself eleven hunger pangs in the same time. The rate in each
of us was, therefore, approximately the same. This rate is
slightly slower than that found in dogs by Boldireff; the difference is
perhaps correlated with the slower rhythm of gastric peristalsis in man
compared with that in the dog. 37
Before hunger was experienced by Washburn the recording
apparatus revealed no signs of gastric activity. Sometimes a rather
tedious period of waiting had to be endured before contractions
occurred. And after they began they continued for a while, then ceased
(see Fig. 38). The feeling of hunger, which was reported while the
contractions were recurring, disappeared as the waves stopped. The
inability of the subject to control the contractions eliminated the
possibility of their being artifacts, perhaps induced by suggestion.
The close concomitance of the contractions with hunger pangs,
therefore, clearly indicates that they are the real source of those
pangs.
Boldireff's studies proved that when the empty stomach is
manifesting periodic contractions, the intestines also are active.
Conceivably all parts of the alimentary canal composed of smooth muscle
share in these movements. The lower esophagus in man is provided with
smooth muscle. It was possible to determine whether this region in
Washburn was active during hunger.
To the esophageal tube a thin-rubber finger-cot (2
centimeters in length) was attached and lowered into the stomach. The
little rubber bag was distended with air, and the tube, pinched to keep
the bag inflated, was gently withdrawn until resistance was felt. The
air was now released from the bag and the tube farther withdrawn about
3 centimeters. The bag was again distended with air at a manometric
pressure of 10 centimeters of water. Inspiration now caused the writing
lever, which recorded the pressure changes, to rise; and a slightly
farther withdrawal of the tube changed the rise, on inspiration, to a
fall. The former position
of the tube, therefore, was above the gastric cavity and
below the diaphragm. In this position
the bag, attached to a float recorder (with chamber 2.3
centimeters in diameter), registered the periodic
oscillations shown in Fig. 39. Though individually more
prolonged than those of the stomach, these contractions, it will be
noted, occur at about the same rate.
This study of hunger, reported by Washburn and myself in
1912, has since been taken up by Carlson of Chicago, and in
observations on a man with a permanent gastric fistula, as well as on
himself and his collaborators, he has fully confirmed our evidence as
to the relation between contractions of the alimentary canal and the
hunger sensation. In a series of nearly a score of interesting papers,
Carlson and his students 38 have greatly amplified our knowledge of the
physiology of the "empty" stomach. Not only are there the contractions
observed by Washburn and myself, but at times these may fuse into a
continuous cramp of the gastric muscle. The characteristic
contractions, furthermore, continue after the vagus nerve supply to the
stomach has been destroyed, and, therefore, are not dependent on the
reception of impulses by way of the cranial autonomic fibres. Recently
Luckhardt and Carlson have brought forward evidence that the blood of a
fasting animal if injected into the vein of a normal animal is capable
of inducing in the latter the condition of
cramp or tetanus in the gastric muscle mentioned above an
effect which does not occur when the blood of a well-fed animal is
injected. It seems possible that a substance exists in the blood which
acts to excite the gastric hunger mechanism. But this point will
require further investigation. With these demonstrations that
contractions are the immediate cause of hunger, most of the
difficulties confronting other explanations are readily obviated. Thus
the sudden onset of hunger and its peculiar periodicity phenomena which
no
other explanation of hunger can account for are at once
explained.
In fever, when bodily material is being most rapidly used,
hunger is absent. Its absence is
understood from an observation made by F. T. Murphy and
myself, 39 that infection, with systemic
involvement, is accompanied by a total cessation of all
movements of the alimentary canal. Boldireff observed that when his
dogs were fatigued the rhythmic contractions failed to appear. Being
"too tired to eat" is thereby given a rational explanation.
A pathological form of the sensation the inordinate hunger
(bulimia) of certain neurotics is in accordance with the well-known
disturbances of the tonic innervation of the alimentary canal in such
individuals.
Since the lower end of the esophagus, as well as the stomach,
contracts periodically in hunger, the reference of the sensation to the
sternum by the ignorant persons questioned by Schiff was wholly
natural. The activity of the lower esophagus also explains why, after
the stomach has been removed, or in some cases when the stomach is
distended with food, hunger can still be experienced. Conceivably the
intestines also originate
vague sensations by their contractions. Indeed, the final
banishment of the modified hunger sensation in the patient with
duodenal fistula, described by Busch, may have been due to the lessened
activity of the intestines when chyme was injected into them.
The observations recorded in this chapter have, as already
noted, numerous points of similarity to Boldireff's observations 40 on
the periodic activity of the alimentary canal in fasting dogs. Each
period of activity, he found, comprised not only wide-spread
contractions of the digestive canal, but also the pouring out of bile,
and of pancreatic and intestinal juices rich in ferments. Gastric juice
was not secreted at these times; when it was secreted and reached the
intestine, the periodic activity ceased. What is the significance of
this extensive
disturbance ? I have elsewhere presented evidence 41 that
gastric peristalsis is dependent on the stretching of gastric muscle
when tonically contracted. The evidence that the stomach is in fact
strongly contracted in hunger i. e., in a state of high tonus has been
presented above.* Thus
* The "empty" stomach and esophagus contain gas (see
Hertz:
Quarterly Journal of Medicine, 1910, iii, p. 378; Mikulicz:
Mittheilungen aus den Grenzgebieten der Medicin und Chirurgie, 1903,
xii, p. 596). They would naturally manifest rhythmic contractions on
shortening tonically on their content.
the very condition which causes hunger and leads to the
taking of food is the condition, when the swallowed food stretches the
shortened muscles, for immediate starting of gastric peristalsis. In
this connection the observations of Haudek and Stigler 42 are probably
significant. They found that the stomach discharges its contents more
rapidly if food is eaten in hunger than if not so eaten. Hunger, in
other words, is normally the signal that the stomach is contracted for
action; the unpleasantness of hunger leads to eating; eating starts
gastric digestion, and abolishes the sensation. Meanwhile the
pancreatic and intestinal juices, as well as bile, have been prepared
in the duodenum to receive the oncoming chyme. The periodic activity of
the alimentary canal in fasting, therefore, is not solely the source of
hunger pangs, but is at the same time an exhibition in the digestive
organs of readiness for prompt attack on the food swallowed by the
hungry animal.
REFERENCES
1 Cannon: The Mechanical Factors of Digestion, London and New
York, 1911, p. 204.
2 Bardier: Richet's Dictionnaire de Physiologie, article
Faini, 1904, vi, p. 1. See, also, Howel: Text-book of Physiology,
fourth edition, Philadelphia and London, 1911, p. 285.
3 See Sternberg: Zentralblatt für Physiologie, 1909,
xxii, p.
653. Similar views were expressed by Bayle in a
thesis presented to the Faculty of Medicine in Paris in 1816.
4 See Hertz: The Sensibility of the Alimentary Canal, London,
1911, p. 38.
5 Schiff: Physiologie de la Digestion, Florence and Turin,
1867, p. 40.
6 Luciani: Das Hungern, Hamburg and Leipzig, 1890, p. 113.
7 Tigerstedt: Nagel's Handbuch der Physiologie, Berlin, 1909,
i, p. 376.
8 Johanson, Landergren, Sonden arid Tigerstedt:
Skandinavisches Archiv für Physiologie, 1897, vii, p. 33.
9 Carrington: Vitality, Easting and Nutrition, New York,
1908, p. 555.
10 Viterbi, quoted by Bardier: Loc. cii.f p. 7.
11 Busch: Archiv für pathologische Anatomic und
Physiologie
und für klinische Medicin, 1858, xiv, p. 147.
12 See Schiff: Loc. cit., p. 37; also Ducceschi; Archivio di
Fisiologia, 1910, viii, p. 579.
13 Lonet: Traite de Physiologie, Paris, 1808, i, p. 23.
14 Ludwig: Lehrbuch der Physiologie des Menschen, Leipzig and
Heidelberg, 1858, ii, p. 584.
15 Maxwell: Journal of Biological Chemistry, 1906-7, ii, p.
194.
16 See Schiff: Loc. cit., p. 49.
17 See Schiff: Loc. cit., p. 31; Bardier; Loc. cit., p. 16.
18 Head: Brain, 1893, xvi, p. 1; 1901, xxiv, p. 345.
19 Nicolai: Ueber die Entstehung des Hungergefühls,
Inaugural
Dissertation, Berlin, 1892, p. 17.
20 Beaumont: The Physiology of Digestion, second edition,
Burlington, 1847, p. 51.
21 Nicolai: Loc. cit., p. 15.
22 Beaumont: Loc. cit., p. 55.
23 Luciani: Archivio di Fisiologia, 1906, iii, p. 54.
Tiedemann long ago suggested that gastric nerves become increasingly
sensitive as fasting progresses. (Physiologie des Menschen, Darmstadt,
1836, iii, p. 22.)
24 Valenti: Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 1910. liii, p.
94.
25 Weber: Wagner's Handworterbuch der Physiologie, 1846, iii
2 , p. 580.
26 Vierordt: Grundriss der Physiologie, Tubingen, 1871, p.
433.
27 Schiff: Loc. cit., p. 33.
28 Luciani: Loc. cit.f p. 542.
29 Valenti: Loc. cit., p. 95.
30 Bettmann: Philadelphia Monthly Medical Journal, 1899, i,
p. 133.
31 Wolff: Dissertation, Giessen, 1902, p. 9.
32 His: Archiv für Anatomie, 1903, p. 345.
33 Boldireff: Loc. cit., p. 1.
34 Boldireff: Loc. cit., p. 96.
35 See Cannon and Lieb: American Journal of Physiology, 1911,
xxix, p. 267.
36 Ducceschi: Archivio per le Scienze Mediche, 197, xxi, p.
154.
37 See Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1903, viii, p.
xxi; 1905, xiv, p. 344.
38 See American Journal of Physiology, 1913, 1914.
39 Cannon and Murphy: Journal of the American Medical
Association, 1907, xlix, p. 840.
40 Boldireff: Loc. cit., pp. 108-111.
41 Cannon: American Journal of Physiology, 1911, xxix, p.
250.
42 Haudek and Stigler: Archiv für die gesammte
Physiologie,
1910, cxxxiii, p. 159.
CHAPTER XIV
THE INTERRELATIONS OF EMOTIONS
Emotions gain expression through, discharges along the
neurones of the autonomic nervous system. The reader will recall that
this system has three divisions the cranial and sacral, separated by
the sympathetic and that when the neurones of the mid-division meet in
any organ the neurones of either of the end divisions, the influence of
the two sets is antagonistic. As previously stated (p. 35), there is
evidence that arrangements exist in the central nervous system for
reciprocal innervation of these antagonistic divisions, just as there
is
reciprocal innervation of antagonistic skeletal muscles. The
characteristic affective states manifested in the working of these
three divisions have been described. Undoubtedly, these states have
correspondents activities and inhibitions in the central neurones. The
question now arises, are the states which appear in opposed divisions
also in opposition?
ANTAGONISM BETWEEN EMOTIONS EXPRESSED IN THE SYMPATHETIC AND
IN THE CRANIAL DIVISIONS OF THE AUTONOMIC SYSTEM
The cranial autonomies, as already shown, is concerned with
the quiet service of building up reserves and fortifying the body
against times of stress. Accompanying these functions are the
relatively mild pleasures of sight and taste and smell of food. The
possibility of existence of these gentle delights of eating and
drinking and also of their physiological consequences is instantly
abolished in the presence of emotions which activate the sympathetic
division. The secretion of saliva, gastric juice, pancreatic juice and
bile is stopped,
and the motions of the stomach and intestines cease at once,
both in man and in the lower animals, whenever pain, fear, rage, or
other strong excitement is present in the organism.
All these disturbances of digestion seem mere interruptions
of the "normal" course of events unless the part they may play in
adaptive reactions is considered. In discussing the operations of the
sympathetic division, I pointed out that all the bodily changes which
occur in the intense emotional states such as fear and fury occur as
results of activity in this division, and are in the highest degree
serviceable in the struggle for existence likely to be precipitated
when these emotions aroused. From this point of view these
perturbations, which so readily seize and dominate the organs that in
quiet times are commonly controlled by the cranial autonomic, are
bodily reactions which may be of the utmost importance to life at times
of critical emergency. Thus are the body's reserves the stored adrenin
and the accumulated sugar called forth for instant service; thus is the
blood shifted to nerves and muscles that may have to bear the brunt of
struggle; thus is the heart set rapidly beating to speed the
circulation; and thus, also, are the activities of the digestive organs
for the time abolished. Just as in war between nations the arts and
industries which have brought wealth and contentment must suffer
serious neglect or be wholly set aside both by the attacker and the
attacked, and all the supplies and energies developed in the period of
peace must be devoted to the present conflict; so, likewise, the
functions which in quiet times establish and support the bodily
reserves are, in times of stress, instantly checked or completely
stopped, and these reserves lavishly drawn upon to increase power in
the attack and in the defense or flight.*
It is, therefore, the natural antagonism between these two
processes in the body between saving
* One who permits fears, worries and anxieties to
disturb the
digestive processes when there is nothing to he done, is evidently
allowing the body to go onto what we may regard as a "war footing,"
when there is no "war" to be waged, no fighting or struggle to be
engaged in.
and expenditure, between preparation and use, between
anabolism and catabolism and the correlated
antagonism of central innervations, that underlie the
antipathy between the emotional states which normally accompany the
processes. The desire for food, the relish of eating it, all the
pleasures of the table, are naught in the presence of anger or great
anxiety. And of the two sorts of emotional states, those which manifest
themselves in the dominant division of the autonomic hold the field
also in consciousness.
ANTAGONISM BETWEEN EMOTIONS EXPRESSED IN THE SYMPATHETIC AND
IN THE SACRAL DIVISIONS OF THE AUTONOMIC SYSTEM
The nervi erigentes are the part of the sacral autonomic in
which the peculiar excitements of sex are expressed. As previously
stated, these nerves are opposed by branches from the sympathetic
division the division which is operated characteristically in the major
emotions.
The opposition in normal individuals between the emotional
states which appear in these two antagonistic
divisions is most striking. Even in animals as low in the
scale as birds, copulation is not performed "until every condition of
circumstance and sentiment is fulfilled, until time, place and partner
all are fit." 1 And among men the effect of fear or momentary anxiety
or any intense emotional interest in causing inhibition of the act can
be supported by cases in the experience of any physician with extensive
practice. Indeed, as Prince 2 has stated, "the suppression of the
sexual instinct by conflict is one of the most notorious experiences of
this kind in everyday life. This instinct cannot be excited during an
attack of fear or anger, and even during moments of its excitation, if
there is an invasion of another strong emotion the sexual instinct at
once is repressed. Under these conditions, as with other instincts,
even habitual excitants can no longer initiate the instinctive
process."
When the acme of excitement is approaching it is probable
that the sympathetic division is also called into activity; indeed, the
completion of the process the contractions of the seminal vesicles and
the prostate, and the subsidence of engorged tissues, all innervated by
sympathetic filaments (see pp. 32, 33) may be due to the overwhelming
of sacral by sympathetic nervous discharges. As soon as this stage is
reached the original feeling likewise has been dissipated.
The other parts of the sacral division which supply the
bladder and rectum are so nearly free from any emotional tone in their
normal reflex functioning that it is unnecessary to consider them
further with reference to emotional antagonisms. Mild affective states,
such as worry and anxiety, can, to be sure, check the activity of the
colon and thus cause constipation. 3 But the augmented activity of
these parts (contraction of the bladder and rectum) in very intense
periods of emotional stress, when the sympathetic division is strongly
innervated, presents a problem of some difficulty. Possibly in such
conditions the orderliness of the
central arrangements is upset, just as it is after tetanus
toxin or strychnine poisoning, and opposed innervations no longer
discharge reciprocally, but simultaneously, and then the stronger
member of the pair prevails. Only on such a basis, at present, can I
offer any explanation for the activity and the supremacy of the sacral
innervation of the bladder and distal colon when the sympathetic
innervation is aroused, as, for example, in great fright.
THE FUNCTION OP HUNGER
A summary in few words of the chief functions typically
performed or supported by each division of the autonomic would
designate the cranial division as the upbuilder and restorer of the
organic reserves, the sacral as the servant of racial continuity, and
the sympathetic as the preserver of the individual. Self-preservation
is primary and essential; on that depends racial continuity, and for
that all the resources of the organism are called forth. Analogously
the sympathetic innervations, when they meet in organs innervated also
by the cranial and sacral divisions, almost without exception
predominate over their opponents. And analogously, also, the emotional
states which are manifested in the sympathetic division and are
characteristically much more intense than those manifested in the other
divisions, readily assume ascendancy also in consciousness.
It is obvious that extended action of the sympathetic
division, abolishing those influences of the cranial division which are
favorable to proper digestion and nutrition, might defeat its own ends.
Interruption of the nutritional process for the sake of
self-preservation through defense or attack can be only temporary; if
the interruption were prolonged, there might be serious danger to the
vigor of the organism from failure to replenish the exhausted stores.
The body does not have to depend on the return of a banished appetite,
however, before its need for restoration is attended to. There is a
secondary and very insistent manner in which the requirement of food is
expressed, and that is through the repeated demands of hunger.
Unlike many other rhythmically repeated sensations, hunger is
not one that anybody becomes accustomed
to and neglects because of its monotony. During the period of
his confinement in the citadel of Magdeburg, the celebrated political
adventurer Baron von Trenck 4 was allowed only a pound and a half of
ammunition bread and a jug of water as his daily ration. "It is
impossible for me to describe to my reader," he wrote in his memoirs,
"the excess of tortures that during eleven months I endured from
ravenous hunger. I could easily have devoured six pounds of bread every
day; and every twenty-four hours, after having received
and swallowed my small portion, I continued as hungry as
before I began, yet I was obliged to wait another twenty-four hours for
a new morsel.
. . . My tortures prevented sleep, and looking into futurity,
the cruelty of my fate seemed to me, if possible, to increase, for I
imagined that the prolongation of pangs like these was insupportable.
God preserve every honest man from sufferings like mine! They were not
to be endured by the most obdurate villain. Many have fasted three
days, many have suffered want for a week or more, but certainly no one
besides myself ever endured it in the same excess for eleven months;
some have supposed that to eat little might become habitual, but I have
experienced the contrary. My hunger increased every day, and of all the
trials of fortitude my whole life has afforded, this eleven months was
the most bitter."*
* In all probability the continued experience of
hunger pangs
reported by Baron von Trenck was due to the repeated eating of amounts
of food too small to satisfy the bodily demand. The reader will recall
that persons who for some time take no food whatever report that the
disagreeable feelings are less intense or disappear after the third or
fourth day (see p. 238).
Thus, although the taking of food may be set in abeyance at
times of great excitement, and the bodily reserves fully mobilized,
that phase of the organism's self-protecting adjustment is limited, and
then hunger asserts itself as an agency imperiously demanding
restoration of the depleted stores.
THE SIMILARITY OF VISCERAL EFFECTS IN DIFFERENT STRONG
EMOTIONS AND SUGGESTIONS AS TO ITS PSYCHOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE
The dominant emotions which we have been considering as
characteristically expressed in the sympathetic division of the
autonomic system are fear and rage. These two emotions ate not unlike.
As James 5 has indicated, "Fear is a reaction aroused by the same
objects that arouse ferocity. . . . We both fear and wish to kill
anything that may kill us; and the question which of the two impulses
we shall follow is usually decided by some one of those collateral
circumstances of the particular case, to be moved by which is the mark
of
superior mental natures." The cornering of an animal when in
the headlong flight of fear may suddenly turn the fear to fury and the
flight to a fighting in which all the strength of desperation is
displayed.
Furthermore, these dominant emotions are states into which
many other commonly milder affective states may be suddenly
transformed. As McDougall 6 has pointed out, all instinctive impulses
when met with opposition or obstruction give place to, or are
complicated by, the pugnacious or combative impulse directed against
the source of the obstruction. A dog will bristle at any attempt to
take away his food, males will fight furiously when provoked by
interference with the satisfaction of the sexual impulse, a man will
forget the conventions and turn hot for combat when there is imputation
against his honor, and a mother all gentle
with maternal devotion is stung to quick resentment and will
make a fierce display of her combative
resources, if anyone intentionally injures her child. In
these instances of thwarted or disturbed instinctive acts the emotional
accompaniments such as the satisfaction of food and of sexual
affection, the feeling of self-pride, and the tender love of a parent
are whirled suddenly into anger. And anger in one is likely to provoke
anger or fear in the other who for the moment is the object of the
strong feeling of antagonism.
Anger is the emotion preeminently serviceable for the display
of power, and fear is often its counterpart.
The visceral changes which accompany fear and rage are the
result of discharges by way of sympathetic
neurones. It will be recalled that these neurones are
arranged for diffuse rather than for narrowly directed effects. So far
as these two quite different emotions are concerned, present
physiological evidence indicates that differences in visceral
accompaniments* are not noteworthy for example, either fear or rage
stops gastric secretion (see pp. 10, 11). There is, indeed, obvious
reason why the visceral changes in fear and rage
should not be different, but rather, why they should be
alike. As already pointed out, these emotions
accompany organic preparations for action, and just because
the conditions which evoke them are likely to result in flight or
conflict (either one requiring perhaps the utmost struggle), the bodily
needs in either response are precisely the same. In discussing the
functioning of the sympathetic division I pointed out that it was
roused to activity not only in fear and rage, but also in pain. The
machinery of this division likewise is operated wholly or partially in
emotions which are usually mild such as joy and sorrow and disgust
* Obvious vascular differences, as pallor or flushing
of the
face, are of little significance. With increase of blood pressure from
vasoconstriction, pallor might result from action of the constrictors
in the face, or flushing might result because constrictors elsewhere,
as, for example, in the abdomen, raised the pressure so high that
facial constrictors are overcome. Such, apparently, is the effect of
adrenin already described (see p. 107)\ Or the flushing might occur
from local vasodilation. That very different emotional states may have
the same vascular accompaniments was noted by Darwin (The Expression of
Emotions in Man and Animals, New York, 1905), who mentioned the pallor
of rage (p. 74) and also of terror (P. 77).
when they become sufficiently intense. Thus, for instance,
the normal course of digestion may be stopped or quite reversed in a
variety of these emotional states. Darwin 7 reports the case of a young
man who on hearing that a fortune had just been left him, became pale,
then exhilarated, and after various expressions of joyous feeling
vomited the half-digested contents of his stomach. Müller 8 has
described the case of a young woman whose lover had broken the
engagement of marriage. She wept in bitter sorrow for several days, and
during that time vomited whatever food she took. And Burton, 9 in his
Anatomy of Melancholy, gives the following instance of the effect of
disgust: "A gentlewoman of the same city saw a fat hog cut up, when the
entrails were opened, and a noisome savour offended her nose, she much
misliked, and would not longer abide; a physician in presence told her,
as that hog, so was she, full of filthy excrements, and aggravated the
matter by some other loathsome instances, insomuch this nice
gentlewoman apprehended it so deeply that she fell forthwith a
vomiting, was so mightily distempered in mind and body, that with all
his art and persuasion, for some months after, he could not restore her
to herself again, she could not forget or remove
the object out of her sight."
In these three cases, of intense joy, intense sorrow and
intense disgust, the influence of the cranial division of the autonomic
has been overcome, digestion has ceased, and the stagnant gastric
contents by reflexes in striated muscles have been violently
discharged. The extent to which under such circumstances other effects
of sympathetic impulses may be manifested, has not, so far as I know,
been ascertained.
From the evidence just given it appears that any high degree
of excitement in the central nervous system, whether felt as anger,
terror, pain, anxiety, joy, grief or deep disgust, is likely to break
over the threshold of the sympathetic division and disturb the
functions of all the organs which that division innervates. It may be
that there is advantage in the readiness with which these widely
different emotional conditions can express
themselves in this one division, for, as has been shown (see
p. 276), occasions may arise when these milder emotions are suddenly
transmuted into the naturally intense types (as fright and fury) which
normally activate this division; and if the less intense can also
influence it, the physiological aspect of the transmutation is already
partially accomplished.
If various strong emotions can thus be expressed in the
diffused activities of a single division of the autonomic the division
which accelerates the heart, inhibits the movements of the stomach and
intestines, contracts the blood vessels, erects the hairs, liberates
sugar, and discharges adrenin it would appear that the bodily
conditions which have been assumed, by some psychologists, to
distinguish emotions from one another must be sought for elsewhere than
in the viscera. We do not "feel sorry because we cry/' as James
contended, but we cry because when we are sorry or overjoyed or
violently angry or full of tender affection when any one of these
diverse emotional states is present there are nervous discharges by
sympathetic
channels to various viscera, including the lachrymal glands.
In terror and rage and intense elation, for example, the responses in
the viscera seem too uniform to offer a satisfactory means of
distinguishing states which, in man at least, are very different in
subjective quality. For this reason I am inclined to urge that the
visceral changes merely contribute to an emotional complex more or less
indefinite, but still pertinent, feelings of disturbance in organs of
which we are not usually conscious. This view that the differential
features of emotions are not to be traced to the viscera is in accord
with the experimental results of Sherrington, 10 who has demonstrated
that emotional responses occur in dogs in which practically all the
main viscera and the great bulk of skeletal muscle have been removed
from subjection to and from influence upon the brain, by severance of
the vagus nerves and the spinal cord. In these animals no alteration
whatever was noticed in the occurrence, under appropriate
circumstances, of characteristic expressions of voice and features,
indicating anger, delight or fear. The argument that these expressions
may have been previously established by afferent impulses from excited
viscera was met by noting that a puppy only nine weeks old also
continued to exhibit the signs of emotional excitement after the brain
was disconnected from all the body except the head and shoulders.
Evidence from uniformity of visceral response and evidence from
exclusion of the viscera are harmonious, therefore, in minimizing
visceral factors as the source of differences in emotional states.*
If these differences are due to other than visceral changes,
why is it not always possible by voluntary
innervations to produce emotions ? We can laugh and cry and
tremble. But forced laughter does not bring happiness, nor forced
sobbing sorrow, and the trembling from cold rouses neither anger nor
fear. The muscle positions and tensions are there, but the experiencing
of such bodily changes does not seem even approximately to rouse
* The paucity of afferent fibres in the
autonomic
system, and the probability of an extremely low degree of sensitiveness
in the viscera (for evidence, see Cannon: The Mechanical Factors of
Digestion, London, 1911, p. 202), likewise support this conclusion.
an emotion in us. Voluntary assumption of an attitude seems
to leave out the "feeling." It is probable,
however, that no attitude which we can assume has all the
elements in it which appear in the complete
response to a stirring situation. But is not this because the
natural response is a pattern reaction, like inborn reflexes of low
order, such as sneezing, in which impulses flash through peculiarly
cooperating neurone groups of the central system, suddenly,
unexpectedly, and in a manner not exactly reproducible by volition, and
thus they throw the skeletal muscles into peculiar attitudes and, if
sufficiently intense, rush out in diffuse discharges that cause tremors
and visceral perturbations?
The typical facial and bodily expressions, automatically
assumed in different emotions, indicate the
discharge of peculiar groupings of neurones in the several
affective states. That these responses occur instantly and
spontaneously when the appropriate "situation," actual or vividly
imagined, is present, shows that they are ingrained in the nervous
organization. At least one such pattern, that of anger, persists after
removal of the cerebral hemispheres the decorticated dog, by growling
and biting when handled, has the appearance of being enraged; 11 the
decerebrate cat, when vigorously stimulated, retracts its lips and
tongue,
stares with dilated pupils, snarls and snaps its jaws. 12 On
the other hand, stroking the hair, whistling and gently calling to
produce a pleased attitude, or yelling to produce fright, have not the
slightest effect in evoking from the decorticated dog signs of joy and
affection or of fear, nor does the animal manifest any sexual feeling.
The absence of bodily indications of these emotions is quite as
significant as the presence of the signs of anger. For since
expressions of anger can persist without the cerebral cortex, there is
little reason
why the complexes of other emotional expressions, if their
"machinery" exists below the cortex, should not also be elicitable.
That they are not elicitable suggests that they require a more
elaborately organized grouping of neurones than does anger possibly
what the cortex, or the cortex in combination with basal ganglia, would
provide.
The contrast between the brevity of the "pseudoaffective
reactions" in the decerebrate cat, though the viscera are still
connected with the central nervous system, and the normal duration of
emotional expression in the dog with the body separated from the head
region, has been used by Sherrington to weigh the importance of the
visceral and other factors. And the evidence which I have given above,
as well as that which he has offered, favors the view that the viscera
are relatively unimportant in an emotional complex, especially in
contributing differential features.
REFERENCES
1 James: Principles of Psychology, New York, 1905, i, p. 22.
2 Prince: The Unconscious, New York, 1914, p. 456.
3 Hertz: Constipation and Allied Intestinal Disorders,
London, 1909, p. 81.
4 v. Trenck: Merkwürdige Lebensgeschichte, Berlin, 1787,
p.
195.
5 James, Loc. cit.t p. 415.
6 McDougall: Introduction to Social Psychology, London, 1908,
p. 72.
7 Darwin: Loc. cit., p. 76.
8 Miiller: Deutsches Archiv für klinische Medicin, 1907,
Ixxxix, p. 434.
9 Burton: The Anatomy of Melancholy (first published in
1621), London, 1886, p. 443.
10 Sherrington: Proceedings of the Royal Society, 1900, Ixvi,
p. 397.
11 Goltz: Archiv für die gesammte Physiologie, 1892, li,
p.
577.
12 Woodworth and Sherrington: Journal of Physiology, 1904,
xxxi, p. 234.
CHAPTER XV
ALTERNATIVE SATISFACTIONS FOR THE FIGHTING EMOTIONS
The uniformity of visceral responses when almost any feelings
grow very intense, and under such conditions the identity of these
responses with those characteristically aroused in the belligerent
emotion of anger or rage and its counterpart, fear, offer interesting
possibilities of transformation and substitution. This is especially
true in the activities of human beings. And because men have devised
such terribly ingenious and
destructive modes of expressing these feelings in war, an
inquiry into the basis for possible substitution
seems not out of place.
SUPPORT FOR THE MILITARIST ESTIMATE OF THE STRENGTH OF THE
FIGHTING EMOTIONS AND INSTINCTS
The business of killing and of avoiding death has been one of
the primary interests of living beings throughout their long history on
the earth. It is in the highest degree natural that feelings of
hostility often burn with fierce intensity, and then, with astonishing
suddenness, that all the powers of the body are called into action for
the strength of the feelings and the quickness of the response measure
the chances of survival in a struggle where the issue may be life or
death. These are the powerful emotions and the deeply ingrained
instinctive reactions which invariably precede combat. They
are the emotions and instincts that sometimes seize upon individuals in
groups and spread like wildfire into larger and larger aggregations of
men, until entire populations are shouting and clamoring for war. To
whatever extent military plans are successful in devising a vast
machine for attack or defense, the energies that make the machine go
are found, in the last analysis, in human beings who, when the time for
action comes, are animated by these surging elemental
tendencies which assume control of their conduct and send
them madly into conflict.
The strength of the fighting instinct in man has been one of
the main arguments used by the militarists in support of preparation
for international strife. They point to the historical fact that even
among highly civilized peoples scarcely a decade passes without a
kindling of the martial emotions, which explode in actual warfare. Such
fighting, they say, is inevitable the manifestation of "biological law"
and, so long as human nature remains unchanged, decision by battle must
be resorted to. They urge, furthermore, that in war
and in the preparations for war important physical qualities
sturdiness, hardihood, and strength for valorous deeds are given
peculiarly favorable opportunities for development, and that if these
opportunities are lacking, lusty youth will give place to weaklings and
mollycoddles. In addition the militarists say that war benefits mankind
by its moral effects. Without war nations become effete, their ideals
become tarnished, the people sink into self-indulgence, their wills
weaken and soften in luxury. War, on the contrary, disciplines
character, it sobers men, it teaches them to be brave and patient, it
renews a true order of values, and its demand for the supreme sacrifice
of life brings forth in thousands an eager response that is the
crowning
glory of the human spirit. As the inevitable expression of a
deep-rooted instinct, therefore, and as a unique means of developing
desirable physical and moral qualities, war is claimed by the
militarists to be a natural necessity. 1
The militarist contention that the fighting instinct is
firmly fixed in human nature receives strong confirmation in the
results of our researches. Survival has been decided by the grim law of
mortal conflict, and the mechanism for rendering the body more
competent in conflict has been revealed in earlier chapters as
extraordinarily per feet and complete. Moreover, the physiological
provisions for fierce struggle are found not only in the bodies of
lower animals, that must hunt and kill in order to live, but also in
human beings.
Since this remarkable mechanism is present, and through
countless generations has served the fundamentally
important purpose of giving momentous aid in the struggle for
existence, the militarists might properly argue that, as with other
physiological processes, bodily harmony would be promoted by its
exercise. Indeed, they might account for the periodic outburst of
belligerent feelings by assuming that these natural aptitudes
require occasional satisfaction.*
GROWING OPPOSITION TO THE FIGHTING EMOTIONS AND INSTINCTS AS
DISPLAYED IN WAR
In spite of the teachings of history that wars have not grown
fewer, and in spite of the militarist argument that war is a means of
purging mankind of its sordid vices, and renewing instead the noblest
virtues, the conclusion that the resort to arms is unavoidable and
desirable is nowadays being strongly contested. The militarists show
only
* Mr. Graham Wallas has made the interesting
suggestion (The
Great Society, New York, 1914, p. 66) that nervous strain and
restlessness due to "baulked disposition" may result from the absence
of circumstances which would call the emotional responses into action.
And he cites Aristotle's theory that pent passions may be released by
represented tragedy and by music.
part of the picture. No large acquaintance with the character
of warfare is necessary to prove that when elemental anger, hate and
fear prevail, civilized conventions are abandoned and the most savage
instincts determine conduct. Homes are looted and burned, women and
children are abominably treated, and many innocents are murdered
outright or starved to death. No bland argument for the preservation of
the manly
virtues can palliate such barbarities. Even when fighting men
are held within the rules, the devices for killing and injuring are now
made so perfect by devilish ingenuity that by the pulling of a trigger
one man can in a few seconds mow down scores of his fellow-creatures
and send them writhing to agony or death. War has become too horrible;
it is conducted on too stupendous a scale of carnage and expenditure;
it destroys too
many of the treasured achievements of the race; it interferes
too greatly with consecrated efforts to benefit all mankind by
discovery and invention; it involves too much suffering among peoples
not directly concerned in the struggle; it is too vastly at variance
with the methods of fair dealing that have been established between man
and man; the human family has become too closely knit to allow some of
its members to bring upon themselves and all the rest poverty and
distress and a long heritage of bitter hatred and resolution to seek
revenge.
All these reasons for hostility to war imply a thwarting of
strong desires in men desires for family happiness, devotion to beauty
and to scholarship, passion for social justice, hopes of lessening
poverty and disease. As was pointed out in the previous chapter, the
feeling of hostility has no definite object to awaken it. It is roused
when there is opposition to what we ardently wish to get. And because
war brings conditions which frustrate many kinds of eagerly sought
purposes, war has roused in men a hostility against itself. There is
then a war against war, a willingness to fight against monstrous
carnage and destruction, that grows in intensity with every war that is
waged.
THE DESIRABILITY OF PRESERVING THE MARTIAL VIRTUES
Although there is increasing opposition to the display of the
fighting emotions and instincts in war, nevertheless the admirable
moral and physical qualities, claimed by the militarists to be the
unique products of war, are too valuable to be lost. As McDougall 2 has
indicated, when the life of ideas becomes richer, and the means we take
to overcome obstructions to our efforts more refined and complex, the
instinct to fight ceases to express itself in its crude natural manner,
save when most intensely excited, and becomes rather a source of
increased energy of action towards the end set by any other instinct;
the energy of its impulses adds itself to and reinforces that of other
impulses and so helps us to overcome our difficulties. In this lies its
great value for civilized man. A man devoid of the pugnacious instinct
would not only be incapable of anger, but would lack this great source
of reserve energy which is called into play in most of us by any
difficulty in our path.
Thus the very efficiency of a war against war, as well as
struggle against other evils that beset civilized
society, rests on the preservation and use of aggressive
feeling and the instinct to attack. From this point of view the
insistence by the militarists that we must accept human nature as we
find it, and that the attempt to change it is foolish, seems a more
justifiable attitude than that of the pacifists who belittle the
fighting qualities and urge that changing them is a relatively simple
process. We should not wish them changed. Even if in the war against
war a means should be established of securing international justice,
and if through cooperative action the decrees of justice were enforced,
so that the occasions which would arouse belligerent emotions and
instincts were much reduced, there would still remain the need of
recognizing their elemental character and their possible usefulness to
society. What is needed is not a suppression of these capacities to
feel and act, but their diversion into other channels where they may
have satisfactory expression.
MORAL SUBSTITUTES FOR WARFARE
"We must make new energies and hardihoods continue the
manliness to which the military mind so faithfully clings. Martial
virtues must be the enduring cement; intrepidity, contempt of softness,
surrender of private interest, obedience to command, must still remain
the rock upon which states are built." Thus wrote William James 3 in
proposing a "moral equivalent for war." This, he suggested, should
consist of such required service in the hard and difficult occupations
as would take the childishness and superciliousness out of our youth
and give them soberer ideas and healthier sympathies with their
fellow-men. He conceived that by proper direction of its education a
people should become as proud of the attainment by the nation of
superiority in any ideal respect as it would be if the nation were
victorious in war. "The martial type of character," he declared, "can
be bred without war. Strenuous honor and disinterestedness abound
elsewhere. Priests and medical men are in a fashion educated to it, and
we should all feel some degree of it imperative if we were conscious of
our work as an obligatory service to the state. We should be owned, as
soldiers are by the army, and our pride would rise accordingly. We
could be poor, then, without humiliation, as army officers now are. The
only thing needed henceforth is to inflame the civic temper as past
history has inflamed the military temper."
Similar ideas have been expressed by others. 4 It has been
pointed out that the great war of mankind is that against pain,
disease, poverty and sin; that the real heroes are not those who
squander human strength and courage in fighting one another, but those
who fight for man against these his eternal foes. War of man against
man, in this view, becomes dissension in the ranks, permitting the
common enemies to strike their most telling blows. These moral
considerations, however, are apart from the main intent of our
discussion. Our earlier inquiry confirmed the belief that the fighting
emotions are firmly rooted in our natures, and
showed that these emotions are intimately associated with
provisions for physical exertion. It is particularly in this aspect of
the discussion of substitutes for war that these studies have
significance.
PHYSICAL SUBSTITUTES FOR WARFARE
The idealization of the state and the devotion of service to
social welfare, which have been suggested as moral substitutes for
military loyalty, leave unanswered the claims of the militarists that
in war and in preparations for war opportunities are offered which are
peculiarly favorable to the development of important physical qualities
bodily vigor, sturdiness, and ability to withstand all manner of
hardships.
In the evidence previously presented, it seems to me there
was a suggestion that offers a pertinent alternative to these claims.
"When the body goes onto what we have called a war footing, the
physiological changes that suddenly occur are all adapted to the
putting forth of supreme muscular and nervous efforts. That was what
primitive battle consisted of, through countless myriads of generations
a fierce physical contest of beast
with beast, and of man with man. Such contests, attended as
they were by the thrill of unpredictable
incidents, and satisfying completely the lust of combat, are
to be contrasted with the dull grind in preparation for modern war, the
monotonous regularity of subservience, the substitution everywhere of
mechanism for muscle, and often the attack on an enemy who lies wholly
unseen.* As
* Lord Wolseley, while commander-in-chief of the
English
forces, in 1897, secured sanction for not displaying the regimental
colors in battle. "It would be madness and a crime," he declared, "to
order any soldier to carry colors into action in the future. You might
quite as well order him to be assassinated. We have had most
reluctantly to abandon a practice to which we attached great
importance, and which, under past and gone conditions of fighting, was
invaluable in keeping alive the regimental spirit upon which our
British troops depended so much." All war has been transformed by the
invention of the far-reaching and fate-dealing rifle and automatic gun,
with which an enemy kills, whose face is not even seen. War is almost
reduced to a mechanical inter change of volleys and salvoes, and to the
intermittent fire of rifles and machine guns, with short rushes at the
last, in which there is no place for the dignity and grace of the
antique battle of the standard. (See London Times, July 31, 1897, p.
12.)
T. F. Millard, the well-known correspondent of the
Russo-Japanese War, wrote as follows of the characteristics of present
day conflicts: "A large part of modern war is on too great a scale to
give much opportunity for individual initiative. Soldiers can rarely
tell what is going on in their immediate vicinity. They cannot always
see the enemy they are firing at, and where they can see the object of
their fire such an important matter as range and even direction cannot
be left to them. . . . Troops are clothed so much alike nowadays that
it is very difficult to distinguish friend from foe at five hundred
yards, and large bodies of troops rarely get that close to each other
in modern war while there is light enough to see clearly. . . . Battery
officers simply see that their guns are handled according to
instructions. They regulate the time, speed, objective and range as
ordered. . . . The effects of the fire are observed by officers
appointed to that duty, stationed at various parts of the field, often
miles and miles apart, and who are in constant communication with the
chief of artillery by telephone." (See Scribner's Magazine, 1905,
xxxvii, pp. 64, 66.)
The testimony of a captain of a German battery engaged
against the French and English in 1914, supports the foregoing claims.
He is reported as saying: "We shoot over those tree tops yonder in
accordance with directions for range and distance which come from
somewhere else over a field telephone, but we never see the men at whom
we are firing. They fire back without seeing us, and sometimes their
shells fall short or go beyond us, and sometimes they fall among us and
kill and wound a few of us. Thus it goes on day after day. I have not
with my own eyes seen a Frenchman or an Englishman unless he was a
prisoner. It is not so much pleasure fighting like this." (See
Philadelphia Saturday Evening Post, December 26, 1914, p. 27.)
Wallas with nice irony has remarked, "The gods in Valhalla
would hardly choose the organization of modern lines of military
communication, as they chose the play of sword and spear, to be the
most exquisite employment of eternity." While it is true that physical
strength can be developed by any form of hard labor, as, for example,
by sawing wood or digging ditches, such labor does not stimulate
quickness, alertness, and
resourcefulness in bodily action. Nor does it give any
occasion for use of the emotional mechanism for reinforcement. If this
mechanism, like other physiological arrangements, is present in the
body for use and previous discussion leaves little doubt of that then
as a means of exercising it and, in addition, satisfying the strong
instinct for competitive testing of strength and physical skill, some
activity more enlivening than monotonous gymnastics and ordered
marching is required. In many respects strenuous athletic rivalries
present, better than modern military service, the conditions
for which the militarists argue, the conditions for which the body
spontaneously prepares when the passion for fighting prevails. As
explained in an earlier chapter, in competitive sports the elemental
factors are retained man is again pitted against man, and all the
resources of the body are summoned in the eager struggle for victory.
And because, under such circumstances, the same physiological
alterations occur that occur in anticipation of mortal combat, the
belligerent emotions and instincts, so far as their bodily
manifestations are concerned, are thereby given complete satisfaction.
THE SIGNIFICANCE OF INTERNATIONAL ATHLETIC COMPETITIONS
For reasons offered above, I venture to lay emphasis on a
suggestion, which has been made before by others, that the promotion of
great international athletic contests, such as the Olympic games, would
do for our young men much that is now claimed as peculiar to the values
of military discipline. The substitution of athletic rivalries for
battle is not unknown. In the Philippine Islands, according to
Worcester, 5 there were no
athletics before the American occupation. The natives soon
learned games from the soldiers. And when the sports reached such
development that competition between towns and provinces was possible,
they began to arouse the liveliest enthusiasm among the people. The
physical development of the participants has been greatly stimulated,
the spirit of fair play and sportsmanship, formerly lacking, has sprung
into existence in
every section of the Islands, and the annual meets between
athletic teams from various provinces are recognized as promoting a
general and friendly understanding among the different Filipino tribes.
The fierce Igarots of Bontoc, once constantly at war with neighboring
tribes, now show their prowess not in head-hunting, but in baseball,
wrestling, and the tug-of-war.*
Is it unreasonable to expect that what has happened in the
Philippine Islands might, by proper education and suggestion, happen
elsewhere in the world! Certainly the interest in athletic contests is
no slight and transient interest. At the time of a great war we know
that news of the games is fully as much demanded as news of the war.
Already in the United States, without special stimulation, the number
of young men engaged in athletic training is estimated as equal to the
number in the standing army. And in England, belief
in the efficacy of athletics as a means of promoting
hardihood and readiness to face stern hazards has found expression in
the phrase that England's battles have been won on the playing fields
of Kugby and of Eton. With the further promotion of international
contests the influence of competitive sports is likely to increase
rather than lessen. Within national boundaries emulation is sure to
stimulate extensively such games as will bring forth the best
representative athletes that the country
* It is reported that when these warriors first
appeared at
the games, each brought his spear, which he drove into the ground
beside him, ready for use. As the nature of the new rivalries became
known, the spears were left behind.
can produce. In one of the high-spirited European nations,
which made a poor showing at the last Olympic meet, thousands of young
men began training for the next meet, under a director imported from
the nation that had made the highest records.
Training for athletic contests is quite as likely to enure
young men to physical hardship and fatigue, is quite as conducive to
the development of bodily vigor, the attainment of alertness and skill
and the practice of self-restraint, as is army life with its
traditional associations and easy license. It may be urged, however,
that an essential element is lacking in all this discussion the
sobering possibility that in war the supreme surrender of life itself
may be required. Death for one's country is indeed glorious. But the
argument that being killed is desirable has little to commend it. When
the strongest and sturdiest are constantly chosen to be fed to the
engines of annihilation, the race is more likely to lose greater values
than it gains from the spectacle of self-sacrifice, however perfect
that may be. Are there not advantages in the conditions of great
athletic rivalries that may compensate for war's most austere demand?
The race of hardy men, to secure
which the militarists urge war, is much more likely to result
from the honoring and preserving of vigorous men in their vigor than it
is from the systematic selection of such men to be destroyed in their
youth.
There are other aspects of international games which strongly
commend them as an alternative to the pursuit of military discipline.
The high standards of honor and fairness in sport; its unfailing
revelation of excellence without distinctions of class, wealth, race or
color; the ease with which it becomes an expression of the natural
feelings of patriotism; the respect which victory and pluckily borne
defeat inspire in competitors and spectators alike; the extension of
acquaintance and understanding which follows from friendly and
magnanimous rivalry among strong men who come together from the ends of
the - earth each of these admirable features of athletic contests
between nations might be enlarged upon. But, as intimated before, these
moral considerations must be left without further mention, as being
irrelevant to the physiological processes with which we are dealing. We
are concerned with the question of exercising the fighting instinct and
thus assuring the physical welfare of the race. The race must
degenerate, the militarists say, if this instinct is not allowed to
express itself in war. This declaration we are in a position to deny,
for the evidence is perfectly clean-cut that the aggressive instincts,
which through aeons of racial experience have naturally and
spontaneously developed vigor and resourcefulness in the body, are
invited by elemental emotions, and that through these emotions energies
are released which are highly useful to great physical effort. No
stupid routine of drill, or any other deadening procedure, will call
these energizing mechanisms into activity.
War and the preparations for war nowadays have become too
machine-like to serve as the best means of preserving and disciplining
these forces. The exhilarating swing and tug and quick thrust of the
big limb muscles have largely vanished. Pressing an electric contact or
bending the trigger finger is a movement altogether too trifling. If,
then, natural feelings must be expressed, if the fighting functions of
the body must be exercised, how much better that these satisfactions be
found in natural rather than in artificial actions, how much more
reasonable that men should struggle for victory in the ancient ways,
one against another,
body and spirit, as in the great games.
REFERENCES
1 See Angell: The Great Illusion, New York and London, 1913,
pp. 159-164.
2 McDougall: Introduction to Social Psychology, London, 1908,
p. 61.
3 James: Memories and Studies, New York, 1911, p. 287.
4 See Perry:: The Moral Economy, New York, 1909, p. 32; and
Drake: Problems of Conduct, Boston, 1914, p. 317.
5 Worcester: The Philippines, Past and Present, New York,
1914, ii, pp. 515, 578.
A LIST OF PUBLISHED RESEARCHES FROM THE PHYSIOLOGICAL
LABORATORY IN HARVARD UNIVERSITY, ON WHICH THE PRESENT ACCOUNT IS
BASED.
1. The Influence of Emotional States on the Functions of the
Alimentary Canal. By W. B. Cannon. American Journal of the Medical
Sciences, 1909, cxxxvii, pp. 480-487.
2. Emotional Stimulation of Adrenal Secretion. By W. B.
Cannon and D. de la Paz. American Journal of Physiology, 1911, xxviii,
pp. 64-70.
3. The Effects of Asphyxia, Hyperpnoea, and Sensory
Stimulation on Adrenal Secretion. By W. B. Cannon and R. G. Hoskins.
Ibid., 1911, xxix, pp. 274-279.
4. Emotional Glycosuria. By W. B. Cannon, A. T. Shohl and W.
S. Wright. Ibid., 1911, xxix, pp. 280-287.
5. A Consideration of Some Biological Tests for Epinephrin.
By R. G. Hoskins. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental
Therapeutics, 1911, iii, pp. 93-99.
6. The Sthenic Effect of Epinephrin upon Intestine. By R. G.
Hoskins. American Journal of Physiology, 1912, xxix, pp. 363-366.
7. An Explanation of Hunger. By W. B. Cannon and A. L.
Washburn. Ibid., 1912, xxix, pp. 441-454.
8. A New Colorimetric Method for the Determination of
Epinephrin. By O. Folin, W. B. Cannon and W. Denis. Journal of
Biological Chemistry, 1913, xiii, pp. 477-483.
9. The Depressor Effect of Adrenalin on Arterial Pressure. By
W. B. Cannon and Henry Lyman, American Journal of Physiology, 1913,
xxxi, pp. 376-398.
10. The Effect of Adrenal Secretion on Muscular Fatigue. By
W. B. Cannon and L. B. Nice. Ibid., 1913, xxxii, pp. 44-60.
11. Fatigue as Affected by Changes of Arterial Pressure. By
C. M. Gruber. Ibid., 1913, xxxii, pp. 222-229.
12. The Threshold Stimulus as Affected by Fatigue and
Subsequent Rest. By C. M. Gruber. Ibid., 1913, xxxii, pp. 438-449.
13. The Fatigue Threshold as Affected by Adrenalin and by
Increased Arterial Pressure. By C. M. Gruber. Ibid., 1914, xxxiii, pp.
335-355.
14. The Emergency Function of the Adrenal Medulla in Pain and
the Major Emotions. By W. B. Cannon. Ibid., 1914, xxxiii, pp. 356-372.
15. The Relation of Adrenalin to Curare and Fatigue in Normal
and Denervated Muscles. By C. M. Gruber. Ibid., 1914, xxxiv, pp. 89-96.
16. The Graphic Method of Recording Coagulation. By W. B.
Cannon and W. L. Mendenhall. Ibid*, 1914, xxxiv, pp. 225-231.
17. The Hastening or Retarding of Coagulation by Adrenalin
Injections. By W. B. Cannon and Horace Gray.
Ibid., 1914, xxxiv, pp. 232-242.
18. The Hastening of Coagulation by Stimulating the
Splanchnic Nerves. By W. B. Cannon and W. L. Mendenhall. Ibid., 1914,
xxxiv, pp. 243-250.
19. The Hastening of Coagulation in Pain and Emotional
Excitement. By W. B. Cannon and W. L. Mendenhall. Ibid. f 1914, xxxiv,
pp. 251-261.
20. The Interrelations of Emotions as Suggested by Recent
Physiological Researches. By W. B. Cannon. American Journal of
Psychology, 1914, xxv, pp. 256-282.
21. The Isolated Heart as an Indicator of Adrenal Secretion
Induced by Pain, Asphyxia and Excitement By W. B. Cannon. American
Journal of Physiology, 1919, 1, pp. ?.
Go Up